1995
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.158
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of changes in relative cue strength on context-dependent recognition.

Abstract: Theoretical analyses and empirical studies address the issue of how context-dependent recognition is affected by changes in the relative strength of retrieval cues. Analyses of global memory models based on K. Murnane and M. P. Phelps' (1994) general context model showed that if context strength is held constant, context effects are predicted to either increase or remain unchanged when item strength increases. In contrast, the outshining hypothesis (S. M. Smith, 1988Smith, , 1994 predicts that context effects … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

10
88
5
1

Year Published

2001
2001
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 65 publications
(104 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
10
88
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The theory states that environmental contextual features are processed at learning and at test unless the ambient environment is suppressed, either because of conceptual processing, or because nonambient contexts are mentally reinstated. The theory is predicated on the idea that remembering depends on the similarity or featural overlap of memory traces and memory probes, a commonly used principle of memory theories (e.g., J. M. Eich, 1982;Hintzman, 1988;Humphreys et al, 1989;Murnane & Phelps, 1995;Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1981). The basic principles proposed herein can be easily accommodated by most current theories of memory and are consistent with the findings of the present meta-analysis.…”
Section: Environmental Context Encoding and Cuingsupporting
confidence: 72%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The theory states that environmental contextual features are processed at learning and at test unless the ambient environment is suppressed, either because of conceptual processing, or because nonambient contexts are mentally reinstated. The theory is predicated on the idea that remembering depends on the similarity or featural overlap of memory traces and memory probes, a commonly used principle of memory theories (e.g., J. M. Eich, 1982;Hintzman, 1988;Humphreys et al, 1989;Murnane & Phelps, 1995;Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1981). The basic principles proposed herein can be easily accommodated by most current theories of memory and are consistent with the findings of the present meta-analysis.…”
Section: Environmental Context Encoding and Cuingsupporting
confidence: 72%
“…These findings contrast with the finding of our metaanalysis that shows that incidental environmental context manipulations reliably affect recognition memory (i.e., measures such as d ¢), rather than cause the same-direction effects found by Murnane and Phelps. In spite of the differences between Murnane and Phelps's studies and those reviewed in the present metaanalysis, at least one f inding appears to link the two. This finding is that, contrary to predictions of our theory, shallower input processing did not increase contextdependent recognition (Murnane & Phelps, 1995;. Our theory predicts that deeper processing of items at input should increase the encoding of item information in memory traces and in memory probes, thereby decreasing context-dependent effects.…”
Section: Environmental Context Encoding and Cuingcontrasting
confidence: 57%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Hintzman & Summers, 1973). Context has also been defined as the physical environment in which an item occurs (e.g., Godden & Baddeley, 1975;Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1994, 1995Murnane, Phelps, & Malmberg, 1999;Smith, Glenberg, & Bjork, 1978). Howard and Kahana (2002) described the context associated with a given item as a composite representation of the semantic features of the items that preceded it on a list.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Clark & Gronrund, 1996)ῌ ̰ΰῳῤῧQQ ῦ̮̮ῡ´῎ῷ̲̲ΰ̯QQ̳Ύῦ῎QΊ`̲ ῲQ ΅QῳῢῨ̮`Ὺῧῧῌ QQΎ̲ ῧῬ̮ῥQῴΎ̲ ῧQQQ̮ΏQ̲ΰΊῡ̮̯ (cf. Clark & Gronrund, 1996) (Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1994, 1995Murnane et al, 1999) (Smith, 1988(Smith, , 1994)῍ ῙῙῡῌ Q̮̮΅ῦQῚῧῌ Q̮̮΅Qῥ̰̰῝`ῢῲ ῠ` (Watkins & Watkins, 1975) Dalton, 1993;Q̯ , 1991;Russo, Ward, Geurts, & Scheres, 1999;Smith, 1986ῐ῎ (Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1995Murnane et al, 1999) (Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1995Murnane et al, 1999) (Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1995Murnane et al, 1999) (Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1995) ῡQ̮̯Ί῭ῠ̮Ῥ῍ Ὶ῭ῥ̮ ῠῌ QῨῥ̮̰̰῞QQῳῶῦQ̳`ΊῨ̯QQῡῧῌ ̯ῦ̮῟῭̰̮ῳῶ̱Ώῲ̲ Q`QQῠ̮Ῥ ῒe.g., Dalton, 1993;Q̯ῌ 1991;Russo et al, 1999;Smith, 1986ΐ῍ ̰QQῳῶ̰ῗQQQῳῶῦ̰̮ῥῌ ̱̲Ὸῼῑῴ̱Ὼ̲ῲῦ̲̯ῤῚ`῏Q̯ῳῶ ῢῌ QQῌ ῦ̮ῌ BGM ῦ̰̮ῥῌ ῧQ̯QQῥQ῞ Ῥῳῶῦ̮῟῭̰̮QQ̯ῳῶῥῲ̲Ί῭ῠ̮Ῥ̮ (e.g., Smith, 1988)ῌ ̯ῦQQῥῧQῐῦῐ̯̮Q ῞ῬῚῢ̮̱QΊ῭ῠ̮Ῥ῍ ̰QQῳῶ̰ῗQQQῳῶ ῧῌ Q̮̮Ύ΅̳̮QῘῤῬῢῌ QῨ̱̱QΎῡῧῳῶ Q̳̮Q῝ῤῘῤῬ̮ ῒῴQQ῏ Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1995Murnane et al, 1999ΐῌ ̯ῦ̰̮ῤQ̮Q̮̮Ύ΅̳QQῡ̰ῌ Q QῳῶῡῧῺ῾ῤῳῶQ̳̮Q῝Ῥ (Canas & Nelson, 1986;…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%