2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.apergo.2005.06.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of display position of a visual in-vehicle task on simulated driving

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
60
0
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 145 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
3
60
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The results in this study did show some differences between the two display configurations, which are in line with earlier research, namely that the display configuration with more centralized information resulted in better driving-and task performance than the spread out display configuration (Horrey, Wickens, & Consalus, 2005;Wittman et al, 2006). According to Tsimhoni and Green (2001), the addition of a secondary task while driving significantly impairs driving performance, which could not be confirmed in this study.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The results in this study did show some differences between the two display configurations, which are in line with earlier research, namely that the display configuration with more centralized information resulted in better driving-and task performance than the spread out display configuration (Horrey, Wickens, & Consalus, 2005;Wittman et al, 2006). According to Tsimhoni and Green (2001), the addition of a secondary task while driving significantly impairs driving performance, which could not be confirmed in this study.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…Having attention on something other than driving, a secondary task, increases the in-vehicle glance durations and is associated with decreased driving performance and increased crash risk (Horrey & Wickens, 2007;Tsimhoni & Green, 2001). Horrey, Wickens, & Consalus (2005) and Wittman et al (2006) declare that if information is acquired from a display located close to the road view, the driving performance is less degraded than for a distant display. Using a Head-Up Display (HUD) can also result in reduced workload, decreased response times, and increased driving comfort (Liu & Wen 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In principle, any of the (three) available sensory modalities could be used for delivering information, for example on CO2 efficiency, to the driver. Supplementary to the information on visual displays provided above, it has to be taken into account that with the emergence of head-up displays (HUD) a viable alternative for information delivery was created [39], [4]. It is evidenced that information presentation via a head-up display results in reduced workload and decreased response times [23].…”
Section: Unemployed Information Reception Capacitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The visual device used to display maps was fixed in the central portion of the windshield and directed at the face of the driver at an angle of approximately 25 degrees between the center of the monitor and the road center. That location and angle were selected as it allow drivers to carry out a visual search moving eyes horizontally without turning the head (Wittmann et al 2006;Burnett et al 2013a). A tablet (Asus ME371MG model), seven-inch screen, video resolution of 1024 x 768 pixels, was used to display the maps.…”
Section: Participants Apparatus and Displaymentioning
confidence: 99%