2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.07.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of salience are both short- and long-lived

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
35
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
1
35
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…& The researcher examines differences in eye movements due to stimulus features and develops or selects stimuli that differ systematically on one or more features (see, e.g., Orquin & Lagerkvist, 2015;Towal, Mormann, & Koch, 2013). & Comparisons are made between different groups of participants to the same stimuli.…”
Section: Inappropriate Comparisonsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…& The researcher examines differences in eye movements due to stimulus features and develops or selects stimuli that differ systematically on one or more features (see, e.g., Orquin & Lagerkvist, 2015;Towal, Mormann, & Koch, 2013). & Comparisons are made between different groups of participants to the same stimuli.…”
Section: Inappropriate Comparisonsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A bottom-up process would, for instance, imply that A is more salient than B, and therefore more likely to attract fixations (Itti & Koch, 2001). A top-down process would imply that A is more relevant than B, consequently attracting more fixations (Orquin & Lagerkvist, 2015).…”
Section: Hidden Defaultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is, of course, a strong claim, and it is not necessarily an accurate description of eye movement control in decision making. We know from both laboratory (Milosavljevic, Navalpakkam, Koch, & Rangel, 2012) and real-world studies (Gidlöf, Anikin, Lingonblad, & Wallin, 2017) that bottomup control plays an important role in guiding the eye movements of decision makers, although direct comparisons suggest that top-down control exerts a larger influence than does bottom-up control (Orquin & Lagerkvist, 2015;Orquin, Bagger, & Loose, 2013; for the opposite finding, see Gidlöf et al, 2017). If this claim is true, any method for influencing top-down control should have a large potential for guiding and influencing people's eye movements.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A salience model takes as input any visual scene and produces a topographical map of the most conspicuous locations, that is, those locations that are brighter, have sharper edges, or different colors than their surroundings. Salience maps have been shown to predict attention in various tasks, such as scene viewing (Parkhurst, Law, and Niebur 2002;Foulsham and Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy Underwood 2008), visual search (Itti and Koch 2000;Rutishauser and Koch 2007), and decision making (Towal, Mormann, and Koch 2013;Orquin and Lagerkvist 2015). In fact, several studies suggest that salience exerts a small but robust effect on fixation likelihood so that decision makers are more likely to fixate on objects with a higher salience (Lohse 1997;Milosavljevic et al 2012;Navalpakkam et al 2012).…”
Section: Visual Saliencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The latter effect is proverbially described as the small print effect, that is, the idea that presenting undesirable information in a smaller font will lower the likelihood of decision makers reading it and taking it into account. One example is small nutrition labels or nutrition labels of low salience that fail to attract attention and influence decision makers in a more healthful direction (Orquin and Lagerkvist 2015;Orquin et al 2016).…”
Section: The Gatekeeping Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%