2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example pairs on novices’ learning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

26
201
4
12

Year Published

2011
2011
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 216 publications
(243 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
26
201
4
12
Order By: Relevance
“…On the other hand, an "in press" study, one that occurred more-or-less concurrently to ours (yet after our hypothesis was formulated), casts doubt on the notion that an alternating condition is better than all examples, at least with respect to regular, non-tutored problem solving. In this study all worked examples were as good as alternating examples/problem solving, with both conditions better than all problems, in terms of both lower cognitive load during learning and higher learning outcomes [11]. Thus, it is clear that the outcome of our study, which will now be described, was not obvious.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…On the other hand, an "in press" study, one that occurred more-or-less concurrently to ours (yet after our hypothesis was formulated), casts doubt on the notion that an alternating condition is better than all examples, at least with respect to regular, non-tutored problem solving. In this study all worked examples were as good as alternating examples/problem solving, with both conditions better than all problems, in terms of both lower cognitive load during learning and higher learning outcomes [11]. Thus, it is clear that the outcome of our study, which will now be described, was not obvious.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…Problem-example pairs allow for noting deficiencies in students' own performance, which may increase their attention to those aspects during example study, but in the study of Van Gog et al (2010), problem-example pairs were less effective for learning than example-problem pairs and examples only and did not lead to better learning than problem solving only. A possible explanation is that students are very often unable to diagnose their own performance deficiencies (for a review, see Bjork 1999).…”
Section: How Should Examples Be Delivered To Optimize Their Effectivementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Worked examples that provide all problem solving steps worked out (solved) for the learner are a widely studied instructional design strategy for reducing cognitive demands in instruction on problem solving (Atkinson, Derry, Renkl, & Wortham, 2000;Biesinger & Crippen, 2010). The effects of different sequences (schedules) of worked examples and practice problems, that are to be solved by the learner, have been examined in several studies (e.g., Leppink, Paas, van Gog, van der Vleuten, & van Merriënboer, 2014; Van Gog, Kester, & Paas, 2011). Transitioning from worked examples to practice problems has generally been found to better foster learning for novices than transitioning from practice problems to worked examples.…”
Section: Brief Overview Of Cognitive Load Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Transitioning from worked examples to practice problems has generally been found to better foster learning for novices than transitioning from practice problems to worked examples. Akin to comparing the transitioning from either worked examples to practice problems with transitioning from practice problems to worked examples (Leppink, et al, 2014;Van Gog, et al, 2011), we compare the transitioning from either feedback for each practice problem step to feedback for an entire practice problem with transitioning from feedback for an entire practice problem to feedback after each problem step in the present study.…”
Section: Brief Overview Of Cognitive Load Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%