1997
DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1997.tb00125.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Eliminating overconfidence in psychodiagnosis: Strategies for training and practice.

Abstract: Research in several domains has revealed that when individuals are asked to estimate the probability that their judgments are correct, they reveal an overconfidence effect. Judgments produced in decision environments such as psychodiyrosis, which are by nature complex and ambiguous, appear to be most vulnerable to overconfidence. By implication, this phenomenon threatens the validity of clinical judgment and subjects clients to risks of flawed diagnoses and unsuitable treatments. The authors propose that effec… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
30
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
2
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Instead, there is a human tendency to be overconfident in the accuracy of our decisions (Harvey, 1997). Research in the area of clinical decision-making offers similar conclusions (Arkes, 1981;Faust & Ziskin, 1988;Smith & Dumont, 1997). In their review of clinical psychological judgment, Wedding and Faust (1989) noted a lack of evidence that accuracy is related to experience, expertise, or confidence in the correctness of predictions.…”
Section: Research Articlementioning
confidence: 75%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Instead, there is a human tendency to be overconfident in the accuracy of our decisions (Harvey, 1997). Research in the area of clinical decision-making offers similar conclusions (Arkes, 1981;Faust & Ziskin, 1988;Smith & Dumont, 1997). In their review of clinical psychological judgment, Wedding and Faust (1989) noted a lack of evidence that accuracy is related to experience, expertise, or confidence in the correctness of predictions.…”
Section: Research Articlementioning
confidence: 75%
“…In particular, research demonstrates strong associations between clinicians' assessments and tribunal decisions (Hilton & Simmons, 2001;Whittemore, 1999). Thus, overconfidence regarding estimates of dangerousness may misdirect the decision-making body (Smith & Dumont, 1997). In the case of overestimated risk (false positive), we may see unwarranted limitation of the patient's civil liberties.…”
Section: Research Articlementioning
confidence: 97%
“…Specifically, the UUP has been reported to be robust to feedback (Hacker et al, 2000;Ferraro, 2010;Ehrlinger et al, 2008). Whether or not the unskilled can improve calibration through experience and feedback, thereby reducing the UUP, is also an important practical question from the perspective of the optimization of education and training procedures (Smith & Dumont, 1997;Stone & Opel, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Overconfidence may have negative consequences (Dunning, Heath, & Suls, 2004), such as medical diagnostic errors (e.g., Smith & Dumont, 1997;Haun et al, 2000) and economic losses due to excessive market entry (e.g., Camerer & Lovallo, 1999) or overexposure to risk (e.g., Malmendier & Tate, 2005). It is, therefore, important to understand how overconfidence, and miscalibration in general, can be reduced.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Overconfidence effects have been demonstrated on a number of clinical tasks, including those in which psychologists are asked to make predictions regarding their clients' prognoses (Smith & Dumont, 1997). Hindsight bias may lead mental health professionals to overestimate their predictive capacities because the correct answers to difficult clinical questions often seem obvious in retrospect.…”
Section: Hindsight Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%