2021
DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13847
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Emotional content overrides spatial attention

Abstract: Selective attention is our capacity to filter or focus on particular features of our environment. We typically think of the top-down aspects of such a process. For example, we can selectively attend to particular regions or objects in our visual environment. However, not all of our attentional selection is driven voluntarily. Some stimuli have the capacity to at least partially override our top-down control mechanisms. For example, visual emotional stimuli signaling threat or reward have often been observed to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the current study we used cut‐outs from larger scenes with intermediate complexity. More complex stimuli (including faces and naturalistic scenes) are common in cognitive‐affective perception and attention research (Alonso‐Prieto et al, 2013; Bekhtereva et al, 2019, 2021; Gruss et al, 2012; Rossion et al, 2012; Schettino et al, 2019; Tebbe et al, 2021; Wieser et al, 2014; Wieser, Miskovic, & Keil, 2016). Besides stimulus complexity, differences in stimulus size may play a role as larger stimuli evoke larger ssVEP amplitudes (Duszyk et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the current study we used cut‐outs from larger scenes with intermediate complexity. More complex stimuli (including faces and naturalistic scenes) are common in cognitive‐affective perception and attention research (Alonso‐Prieto et al, 2013; Bekhtereva et al, 2019, 2021; Gruss et al, 2012; Rossion et al, 2012; Schettino et al, 2019; Tebbe et al, 2021; Wieser et al, 2014; Wieser, Miskovic, & Keil, 2016). Besides stimulus complexity, differences in stimulus size may play a role as larger stimuli evoke larger ssVEP amplitudes (Duszyk et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the current study we used cut-outs from larger scenes with intermediate complexity. More complex stimuli (including faces and naturalistic scenes) are common in cognitive-affective perception and attention research (Alonso-Prieto et al, 2013;Bekhtereva et al, 2019Bekhtereva et al, , 2021Gruss et al, 2012;Rossion et al, 2012;Schettino et al, 2019;Tebbe et al, 2021;Wieser et al, 2014;Wieser, Miskovic, et al, 2016). Besides stimulus complexity, differences in stimulus size may play a role as larger stimuli evoke larger ssVEP amplitudes (Duszyk et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This practical difficulty could be circumvented through the use of steady‐state visual‐evoked potentials (ssVEPs) in combination with video presentation. Just as instructed, spatial attention toward flickering stimuli enhances ssVEP amplitude at the attended flicker frequency (Hillyard et al., 1997) and motivated attention enhances ssVEP amplitude during emotional relative to neutral flickering scene perception (Bekhtereva & Müller, 2015, Bekhtereva et al., 2021; Keil et al., 2003, 2005). Conversely, if nonflickering scenes are presented in combination with a competing flickering visual stimulus, ssVEP amplitude is reduced during emotional relative to neutral scene perception (Deweese et al., 2014; Müller et al., 2008; Wieser et al., 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%