2016
DOI: 10.1037/emo0000125
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Emotional restraint is good for men only: The influence of emotional restraint on perceptions of competence.

Abstract: The present research investigated the notion that passionate restraint or "manly emotion" is a relevant emotion norm not only for men but also for women in modern Western society (MacArthur & Shields, 2015). For this, 2 studies were conducted to assess whether restraint in emotional reactivity is perceived as a sign of both emotional and general competence. Restraint was induced by delaying the onset of the emotional reaction to a purported emotion elicitor. The results show that men were indeed rated as both … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
13
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
5
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, priming participants with words related to cooperation and informing them that they were about to work together with an outgroup member was sufficient to eliminate group biases in emotion judgments. The finding is line with other research showing that motivational factors impact emotion decoding accuracy (Thibault et al, 2006) as well as empathic reactions in the form of facial mimicry (Hess et al, 2016). Here, changing participants' mindset by activating cooperation goals was sufficient to erase intergroup biases in ratings of expression genuineness.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, priming participants with words related to cooperation and informing them that they were about to work together with an outgroup member was sufficient to eliminate group biases in emotion judgments. The finding is line with other research showing that motivational factors impact emotion decoding accuracy (Thibault et al, 2006) as well as empathic reactions in the form of facial mimicry (Hess et al, 2016). Here, changing participants' mindset by activating cooperation goals was sufficient to erase intergroup biases in ratings of expression genuineness.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…However, Warner and Shields (2007) note that restrained and controlled expressions of sadness in men are perceived relatively positively. In fact, signs of restraint might even be considered indicative of emotional intelligence in men (Hess et al, 2016). As such, low-intensity sadness expressions might have been deemed appropriate, especially in a sports context, where tears are more acceptable overall (MacArthur & Shields, 2015).…”
Section: Facial Emgmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When men narrate from the victim viewpoint, they may be more likely to inhibit sadness because gender socialization discourages admitting vulnerability (Winterheld, 2017). Men may feel more comfortable expressing sadness when they do not meet their partner's needs, because they prefer to view themselves as in control and capable of fixing the problem (Hess, Shlomo, & Shlomo, 2015). In contrast, women may be comfortable expressing sadness regardless of viewpoint because they are socialized to perceive sadness as gender appropriate (Brody et al, 2002;Feiring et al, 2017).…”
Section: Constructive Interpretationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This approach neglects informative aspects of expressive signals (see e.g., Ambadar et al, 2005 ; Krumhuber et al, 2013 ). Temporal characteristics only evident in dynamic displays impact both on the labeling of expressions and on the inferences about the expresser drawn from them ( Krumhuber et al, 2013 ; Hess et al, 2016a ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%