2011
DOI: 10.1037/a0020938
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Empathy gaps for social pain: Why people underestimate the pain of social suffering.

Abstract: In 5 studies, the authors examined the hypothesis that people have systematically distorted beliefs about the pain of social suffering. By integrating research on empathy gaps for physical pain (Loewenstein, 1996) with social pain theory (MacDonald & Leary, 2005), the authors generated the hypothesis that people generally underestimate the severity of social pain (ostracism, shame, etc.)--a biased judgment that is only corrected when people actively experience social pain for themselves. Using a social exclusi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
87
1
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 114 publications
(93 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
3
87
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Respondents were phoned within 48 h after the incident occurred, granting our results high validity. Generally, future emotions (and related behavior) are hard to imagine when people are in a relatively unemotional state (Loewenstein 2005;Nordgren, Banas, and MacDonald 2011). Measuring behavior in real emergency situations therefore provides increased opportunities to actually assess emotional effects.…”
Section: Conclusion and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Respondents were phoned within 48 h after the incident occurred, granting our results high validity. Generally, future emotions (and related behavior) are hard to imagine when people are in a relatively unemotional state (Loewenstein 2005;Nordgren, Banas, and MacDonald 2011). Measuring behavior in real emergency situations therefore provides increased opportunities to actually assess emotional effects.…”
Section: Conclusion and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In tum, participants overestimated how willing others would be to perform and underestimated how much others would demand to be paid in the same way (Van Boven, Loewenstein, & Dunning, 2005), The general notion of using one's own "cold" unemotional state as a proxy to judge someone else's "hot" emotional state has been shown across a variety of related contexts. For example, people who are not hungry judge others who eat large quantities harsher than do people who are hungry (Nordgren, van der Pligt, & van Harreveld, 2006), and people who find it easy (vs. hard) to "fit in" underestimate how much social exclusion hurts (Nordgren, Banas, &MacDonald, 2011). Accordingly, consistent with the view that underexposure to emotional situations causes people to underestimate the impact of emotions on others, providing people with a "sample" of an affective state helps reduce these gaps in empathy and understanding.…”
Section: Emotional Perspective Taking and The Traditional Benefits Ofmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…In addition, a robust literature focused on negotiation identifies not only the need for disputants to cultivate a positive relationship over time, but to also identify optimal, win-win solutions where possible as they allocate scarce resources (Galinsky, Maddux, Gilin, & White, 2008;Trötschel, Hüffmeier, Loschelder, Schwartz, & Gollwitzer, 2011). Other important studies also suggest that SPT plays an important role in improving these outcomes between individuals and groups as well as at the intergroup level (Nordgren, Banas, & MacDonald, 2011;Paluck, 2010). …”
Section: The Effects Of Different Types Of Social Perspective Takingmentioning
confidence: 99%