1986
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.12.4.496
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Estimates of frequency: Attribute or attribution?

Abstract: Subjects in two experiments were asked to estimate how often items had appeared in a studied list. If the estimates are based on the value of a frequency attribute, the implicit question is, "How many times did you think of this item during study?" If, on the other hand, estimates are memorial attributions, the implicit question is, "How many study episodes does this item now make you think of?" In Experiment 1, subjects studied items that were repeated with the same or a different partner on each appearance. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
45
0

Year Published

1988
1988
2005
2005

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
2
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast to Lichtenstein et al's (1978) view, which assumes that people have biased knowledge of risk frequencies because of "disproportionate exposure, memorability, or imaginability of various events" (p. 551), the regressed-frequency mechanism assumes that people's frequency knowledge is roughly accurate except for the estimates' tendency to regress toward the mean. It should be noted that not only is this tendency akin to the primary bias observed by Lichtenstein et al but also it is ubiquitous in studies of other types of frequency judgments (e.g., Begg, Maxwell, Mitterer, & Harris, 1986;Greene, 1984;Hintzman, 1969Hintzman, , 1988Sedlmeier et al, 1998;Shanks, 1995;Williams & Durso, 1986;Zacks & Hasher, 2002).…”
Section: Direct Encodingmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…In contrast to Lichtenstein et al's (1978) view, which assumes that people have biased knowledge of risk frequencies because of "disproportionate exposure, memorability, or imaginability of various events" (p. 551), the regressed-frequency mechanism assumes that people's frequency knowledge is roughly accurate except for the estimates' tendency to regress toward the mean. It should be noted that not only is this tendency akin to the primary bias observed by Lichtenstein et al but also it is ubiquitous in studies of other types of frequency judgments (e.g., Begg, Maxwell, Mitterer, & Harris, 1986;Greene, 1984;Hintzman, 1969Hintzman, , 1988Sedlmeier et al, 1998;Shanks, 1995;Williams & Durso, 1986;Zacks & Hasher, 2002).…”
Section: Direct Encodingmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…Any of a number of underlying processes could operate (cf. Begg, Maxwell, Mitterer, & Harris, 1986;Jonides & Naveh-Benjamin, 1987). The present investigations identify both encoding and retrieval circumstances that influence adoption of an availability strategy.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies have demonstrated that cued recall and pair recognition are greater following interactive rather than separate imagery, whereas item recognition is better for words imagined separately than for words imagined interactively (Bain & Humphreys, 1988;Begg, 1979;McGee, 1980). In addition, Begg, Maxwell, Mitterer, and Harris (1986) found that judgments of item frequency were more accurate for separately imagined items than for items that were imagined interactively. These results suggest a tradeoff between the encoding of item and associative information.…”
Section: Previous Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%