Both genetic drift and natural selection cause the frequencies of alleles in a population to vary over time. Discriminating between these two evolutionary forces, based on a time series of samples from a population, remains an outstanding problem with increasing relevance to modern data sets. Even in the idealized situation when the sampled locus is independent of all other loci, this problem is difficult to solve, especially when the size of the population from which the samples are drawn is unknown. A standard x 2 -based likelihood-ratio test was previously proposed to address this problem. Here we show that the x 2 -test of selection substantially underestimates the probability of type I error, leading to more false positives than indicated by its P-value, especially at stringent P-values. We introduce two methods to correct this bias. The empirical likelihood-ratio test (ELRT) rejects neutrality when the likelihoodratio statistic falls in the tail of the empirical distribution obtained under the most likely neutral population size. The frequency increment test (FIT) rejects neutrality if the distribution of normalized allele-frequency increments exhibits a mean that deviates significantly from zero. We characterize the statistical power of these two tests for selection, and we apply them to three experimental data sets. We demonstrate that both ELRT and FIT have power to detect selection in practical parameter regimes, such as those encountered in microbial evolution experiments. Our analysis applies to a single diallelic locus, assumed independent of all other loci, which is most relevant to full-genome selection scans in sexual organisms, and also to evolution experiments in asexual organisms as long as clonal interference is weak. Different techniques will be required to detect selection in time series of cosegregating linked loci. P OPULATION geneticists typically seek to understand the forces responsible for patterns observed in contemporaneous samples of genetic data, such as the nucleotide differences fixed between species, polymorphisms within populations, and the structure of linkage disequilibrium. Recently, however, there has been a rapid increase in the availability of dynamic data, where the frequencies of segregating alleles in an evolving population are monitored through time, both in laboratory experiments (Hegreness et al. 2006;Bollback and Huelsenbeck 2007;Barrick et al. 2009;Lang et al. 2011;Orozco-terWengel et al. 2012;Lang et al. 2013) and in natural populations (Barrett et al. 2008;Reid et al. 2011;Denef and Banfield 2012;Winters et al. 2012;Daniels et al. 2013;Maldarelli et al. 2013; Pennings et al. 2013). One important question is whether the changes in allele frequencies observed in such data are the result of natural selection or are simply consequences of genetic drift or sampling noise. In principle, it seems that dynamic data should provide researchers with more power to detect and quantify selective forces while avoiding the assumptions of stationarity that are required...