1989
DOI: 10.3758/bf03210858
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Estimating psychometric functions in forced-choice situations: Significant biases found in threshold and slope estimations when small samples are used

Abstract: When a theoretical psychometric function is fitted to experimental data (as in the obtaining of a psychophysical threshold), maximum-likelihood or probit methods are generally used. In the present paper, the behavior of these curve-fitting methods is studied for the special case of forcedchoice experiments, in which the probability of a subject's making a correct response by chance is not zero. A mathematical investigation of the variance of the threshold and slope estimators shows that, in this case, the accu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
21
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
3
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If two parameters are estimated, i.e. threshold and slope of the psychometric function, O'Regan and Humbert (1989) found that small samples (100 data points simulated with n = 10 number of presentations at N = 10 stimulus values, method of constant stimuli) produce both, low precision and biased estimates. These results were obtained for either maximum-likelihood and probit analysis and they are in accordance with the study of McKee et al (1985).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If two parameters are estimated, i.e. threshold and slope of the psychometric function, O'Regan and Humbert (1989) found that small samples (100 data points simulated with n = 10 number of presentations at N = 10 stimulus values, method of constant stimuli) produce both, low precision and biased estimates. These results were obtained for either maximum-likelihood and probit analysis and they are in accordance with the study of McKee et al (1985).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…O'Regan and Humbert (1989; see also Brand & Kollmeier, 2002) carried out a theoretical analysis elucidating the locations where three levels should be placed to minimize the asymptotic variance of maximum likelihood estimates of threshold and slope in the simplest case. Hill (2001a, 2001b) used simulations to evaluate seven MOCS plans that differed as to the spacing of six stimulus levels and their location relative to that of Ψ, showing that some of these plans are more prone to bias or imprecise estimation.…”
Section: Conventional Mocsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The tight association between estimating Ψ and using MOCS perhaps developed due to lack of alternative methods, but the link currently continues to be reinforced by the fact that even the most recent explicit attempts to determine how best to estimate Ψ either have only compared variants of MOCS or have only proposed or evaluated statistical criteria or algorithms to deal with MOCS data (Foster & Bischof, 1991;Lam, Mills, & Dubno, 1996;Maloney, 1990;Miller & Ulrich, 2001;O'Regan & Humbert, 1989;Treutwein & Strasburger, 1999;Wichmann & Hill, 2001a, 2001b.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When wishing to estimateã andb jointly, Wetherill (1963) proposed minimizing the product of their variances leading to sweetpoints satisfying WðxÞ 2 f0:176; 0:824g; he found that there was no benefit in probing at WðxÞ ¼ 0:5 in addition to these two sweetpoints. O'Regan and Humbert (1989) generalized this work and determined the sweetpoint locations for estimatingã and/orb when c 6 ¼ 0 in Eq. (1).…”
Section: B Methods For Estimating the Pfmentioning
confidence: 99%