2015
DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000109
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evidence for negative feature guidance in visual search is explained by spatial recoding.

Abstract: Theories of attention and visual search explain how attention is guided toward objects with known target features. But can attention be directed away from objects with a feature known to be associated only with distractors? Most studies have found that the demand to maintain the to-be-avoided feature in visual working memory biases attention toward matching objects rather than away from them. In contrast, Arita, Carlisle, and Woodman (2012) claimed that attention can be configured to selectively avoid objects … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
102
3

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(116 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
11
102
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This is consistent with the finding that the presentation of verbal labels of objects speeds their entry in to awareness (Lupyan & Ward, 2013) and orients attention (Spivey, Tyler, Eberhard, & Tanenhaus, 2001), as well as findings that visually specific templates guide attention better than more abstract templates (Hout & Goldinger, 2014;Maxfield & Zelinsky 2012;Vickery et al, 2005). Furthermore, it is consistent with findings that negative information tends not to guide attention in visual search (Beck & Hollingworth, 2015;Becker, Hemsteger, & Peltier, 2016;Moher & Egeth, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…This is consistent with the finding that the presentation of verbal labels of objects speeds their entry in to awareness (Lupyan & Ward, 2013) and orients attention (Spivey, Tyler, Eberhard, & Tanenhaus, 2001), as well as findings that visually specific templates guide attention better than more abstract templates (Hout & Goldinger, 2014;Maxfield & Zelinsky 2012;Vickery et al, 2005). Furthermore, it is consistent with findings that negative information tends not to guide attention in visual search (Beck & Hollingworth, 2015;Becker, Hemsteger, & Peltier, 2016;Moher & Egeth, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Much like the impossibility of complying with a request to not think of a white bear, some have argued that trial-by-trial adjustments of spatial suppression can only be achieved reactively (Moher & Egeth, 2012;Tsal & Makovski, 2006). Note that distractors in those studies were defined based on features rather than locations (see Anderson & Folk, 2012;Beck & Hollingworth, 2015). Here, probe identificationresults from Experiment 2 areconsistent with trial-by-trial shifts in proactive suppression.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 70%
“…Therefore, it does not appear that participants acquired a template for rejection, contrary to the findings of Arita et al (2012). However, as Beck and Hollingworth (2015) point out, the inhibitory benefit found by Arita et al (2012) was very likely supported by the fact that participants could rely on a simple spatial template. This was because all of the to-be-ignored items in their study were grouped in one hemifield, while the other items were located in the opposite hemifield.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 83%