2021
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000983
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Examining variability in the processing of agreement in novice learners: Evidence from event-related potentials.

Abstract: The present study examines both properties of the language and properties of the learner to better understand variability at the earliest stages of second language (L2) acquisition. We used event-related potentials, an oral production task, and a battery of individual differences measures to examine the processing of number and gender agreement in two groups of low-proficiency English-speaking learners of Spanish who were tested in multiple sessions. The results showed an advantage for number, the feature also… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

4
13
2

Year Published

2022
2022
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 111 publications
4
13
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In line with our findings, L2 learners in some of these previous studies have also shown anterior positivity effects, for example, for phrase structure in instructed contexts (Bowden et al., 2013; Faretta‐Stutenberg & Morgan‐Short, 2018; Morgan‐Short, Steinhauer et al., 2012). However, at this point it is not clear what variables may have led to more anterior distributions in our results as compared to other studies in which instructed L2 learners with similar levels of experience showed P600s with more typical central‐posterior distributions (e.g., Gabriele et al., 2021; Tanner et al., 2013). Appendix S9 in the online Supporting Information provides a more detailed discussion of these anterior P600 effects.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 98%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In line with our findings, L2 learners in some of these previous studies have also shown anterior positivity effects, for example, for phrase structure in instructed contexts (Bowden et al., 2013; Faretta‐Stutenberg & Morgan‐Short, 2018; Morgan‐Short, Steinhauer et al., 2012). However, at this point it is not clear what variables may have led to more anterior distributions in our results as compared to other studies in which instructed L2 learners with similar levels of experience showed P600s with more typical central‐posterior distributions (e.g., Gabriele et al., 2021; Tanner et al., 2013). Appendix S9 in the online Supporting Information provides a more detailed discussion of these anterior P600 effects.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 98%
“…A similar overall picture has emerged for L2 processing of the two linguistic structures that were examined in our study—phrase structure and subject‐verb agreement. For subject‐verb agreement, P600 effects (a) have been evidenced in first‐, second‐, and third‐year L2 university learners (Gabriele et al., 2021; Tanner, McLaughlin, Herschensohn, & Osterhout, 2013), although the effects are smaller or vary more in first‐year L2 learners and (b) have been found to emerge after relatively short periods of training in learners at relatively low proficiency levels (Deng, Zhou, Bi, & Chen, 2015). P600 effects have also been found for syntactic phrase structure conditions in both low and high proficiency L2 learners (Bowden, Steinhauer, Sanz, & Ullman, 2013; Erdocia, Zawiszewski, & Laka, 2014; Faretta‐Stutenberg & Morgan‐Short, 2018; Fromont, Royle, & Steinhauer, 2020; Morgan‐Short, Steinhauer, Sanz, & Ullman, 2012; each of these studies used a balanced design—see Stimuli section below).…”
Section: Background Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…First, we point out that using plural subjects would not have worked because third‑person plural DPs in Spanish agree with first‑ and second‑person plural verbs, a process called unagreement (e.g., Las viudas lloramos/lloráis “[we/you‑ 2ND‑PL ] the widows cry‑ 1ST-PL /cry- 2ND-PL ”) (e.g., Höhn, 2016; Hurtado, 1985), and previous work by Mancini et al (2011, 2019) has shown that native speakers treat these sentences differently from outright person violations (i.e., they do not elicit a P600). Most importantly, while previous ERP studies manipulating L1‑L2 similarity have consistently shown an advantage for L2 features instantiated in the L1 (e.g., Alemán Bañón et al, 2014; Bond et al, 2011; Gabriele et al, in press; Gillon‑Dowens et al, 2010, 2011) they have consistently found no advantage for shared morphological instantiations of common features (e.g., Alemán Bañón et al, 2014; Bond et al, 2011; Gabriele et al, in press). For example, Bond et al (2011) and Gabriele et al (in press) examined L1‑English L2‑Spanish learners’ brain responses to subject‑verb number agreement with third‑person singular subjects (instantiated in English) and noun‑adjective number agreement (unique to Spanish), and found no facilitation for the former.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Most importantly, while previous ERP studies manipulating L1‑L2 similarity have consistently shown an advantage for L2 features instantiated in the L1 (e.g., Alemán Bañón et al, 2014; Bond et al, 2011; Gabriele et al, in press; Gillon‑Dowens et al, 2010, 2011) they have consistently found no advantage for shared morphological instantiations of common features (e.g., Alemán Bañón et al, 2014; Bond et al, 2011; Gabriele et al, in press). For example, Bond et al (2011) and Gabriele et al (in press) examined L1‑English L2‑Spanish learners’ brain responses to subject‑verb number agreement with third‑person singular subjects (instantiated in English) and noun‑adjective number agreement (unique to Spanish), and found no facilitation for the former. Other studies have even found a disadvantage for contexts where the L1 overtly marks agreement.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%