2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feeling good and feeling truth: The interactive effects of mood and processing fluency on truth judgments

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
61
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
6
61
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Accordingly, speakers and writers in a negative mood tend to display greater adherence to the conversational maxims of quantity, relevance, quality, and manner. This effect is conceptually consistent with other evidence showing that people in a negative mood tend to use more concrete and specific mental categories, are less likely to apply stereotypes and scripts, are better at distinguishing between central and peripheral information, and are less subject to reconstructive memory biases (Bless, Clore, Schwarz, Golisano, Rabe, & Wölk, 1996;Bless, Schwarz, & Wieland, 1996;Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Süsser, 1994;Forgas, 1998Forgas, , 1999Forgas, , 2007Forgas, , 2011Forgas, Goldenberg, & Unkelbach, 2009;Forgas, Vargas, & Laham, 2005;Koch & Forgas, 2012;Storbeck & Clore, 2005;Unkelbach et al, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Accordingly, speakers and writers in a negative mood tend to display greater adherence to the conversational maxims of quantity, relevance, quality, and manner. This effect is conceptually consistent with other evidence showing that people in a negative mood tend to use more concrete and specific mental categories, are less likely to apply stereotypes and scripts, are better at distinguishing between central and peripheral information, and are less subject to reconstructive memory biases (Bless, Clore, Schwarz, Golisano, Rabe, & Wölk, 1996;Bless, Schwarz, & Wieland, 1996;Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Süsser, 1994;Forgas, 1998Forgas, , 1999Forgas, , 2007Forgas, , 2011Forgas, Goldenberg, & Unkelbach, 2009;Forgas, Vargas, & Laham, 2005;Koch & Forgas, 2012;Storbeck & Clore, 2005;Unkelbach et al, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Consistent with most past research showing that mood effects on cognition and communication are generally subconscious (Forgas, , , ; Forgas & Koch, ), we also found that self‐rated adherence to Grice's maxims was unaffected by self‐rated mood, suggesting an automatic, subconscious mechanism. Instead, speakers' ratings of their compliance with Grice's norms were more informed by their processing fluency experience, a subconscious meta‐cognitive cue that has been shown to influence several types of self‐related and other‐related judgments (Koch & Forgas, ; for a review, see Alter & Oppenheimer, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The Interactive Effects of Mood and Perceptual Fluency on Truth Judgements: Negative Mood Significantly Reduced the Tendency for People to Rely on Visual Fluency as a Truth Cue (after Koch & Forgas, ).…”
Section: Judgemental Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To the best of our knowledge, there are only two studies that directly investigate which variables moderate fluency effects on judgments of truth that are independent of repetition. Specifically, Koch and Forgas () showed that negative mood eliminated the truth effect compared with positive or neutral mood. Research by Hansen, Dechêne, and Wänke () reveals that perceptual fluency affects judgments of truth when the discrepancy between actual and expected fluency is high and not low (for an overview of moderators of other fluency effects, see Alter & Oppenheimer, ; Greifeneder, Bless, & Pham, ; see also Whittlesea & Williams, ).…”
Section: Moderators Of Fluency Effects In Judgments Of Truthmentioning
confidence: 99%