2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fooled by the brain: Re-examining the influence of neuroimages

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

4
34
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
4
34
1
Order By: Relevance
“…At the same time, our results also question the general ‘seductive allure of neuroscience explanations’ (Weisberg et al, , p. 470) and imply that findings from the general literature about the effects of neuroscience information (Weisberg et al, ) have to be reconsidered accordingly. In fact, our study is in contrast with Weisberg et al () and more consistent with very recent research that found no effects of neuroimages (e.g., Hook & Farah, ; Schweitzer, Baker, & Risko, ). The differential results of our study suggest that experience and training background play an important role in the decision process, at least concerning the influence of neuroscience information as part of the promotion strategy.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…At the same time, our results also question the general ‘seductive allure of neuroscience explanations’ (Weisberg et al, , p. 470) and imply that findings from the general literature about the effects of neuroscience information (Weisberg et al, ) have to be reconsidered accordingly. In fact, our study is in contrast with Weisberg et al () and more consistent with very recent research that found no effects of neuroimages (e.g., Hook & Farah, ; Schweitzer, Baker, & Risko, ). The differential results of our study suggest that experience and training background play an important role in the decision process, at least concerning the influence of neuroscience information as part of the promotion strategy.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…A seminal study showed that displaying colorful renderings from brain scans, relative to simple bar graphs or plain text, led individuals to attribute more scientific merit to cognitive research (McCabe and Castel, 2008). Thereafter, however, some accounts questioned the import of these initial findings (Farah and Hook, 2013)—citing shortcomings in the original methodology and inability to replicate (Baker et al, 2013; Hook and Farah, 2013; Michael et al, 2013; Schweitzer et al, 2013). And yet, the quality of brain images may mediate their sway over critical reasoning: compared to tame graphical representations of the brain, images that were more three-dimensional and tangible increased the perceived quality of neuroscience information (Keehner et al, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For a lay audience, studies have shown that neuroscience research is judged to be more scientifically credible [78] and more understandable [79] when it is accompanied by brain images rather than by bar graphs or more abstract topographical maps. Although other studies fail to replicate a general effect of neuroimagery on scientific persuasiveness [80,81], there appear to be at least some limited contexts [82] in which neuroimagery does add gravitas to scientific arguments. In light of results showing that imagery depicting the 3D geometric structure of a brain (e.g., Figure 6B) is more scientifically convincing than a flattened topographic map (e.g., Figure 6D) for lay audiences [83], one conjecture is that lay audiences are naively interpreting the 3D, object-like depictions as directly capturing and displaying brain activity [8,9].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%