2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.11.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Formal Neurosemantics. Logic, meaning and composition in the Brain

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Substantially fewer studies have examined how the brain processes compositional aspects of meaning (for reviews see Pylkkänen et al, 2011; Panizza, 2012) and how context and discourse interact with meaning (see Van Berkum, 2009). Scalar implicatures offer a promising test case for these issues, given that they represent an aspect of meaning that is composed in concert with semantic meaning and that the generation of scalar implicatures is strongly affected by context and expectations about speakers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Substantially fewer studies have examined how the brain processes compositional aspects of meaning (for reviews see Pylkkänen et al, 2011; Panizza, 2012) and how context and discourse interact with meaning (see Van Berkum, 2009). Scalar implicatures offer a promising test case for these issues, given that they represent an aspect of meaning that is composed in concert with semantic meaning and that the generation of scalar implicatures is strongly affected by context and expectations about speakers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent investigations suggest that the patterns of brain activation elicited by violations of real-world plausibility are not the same as those elicited by linguistically-motivated abstract operations such as semantic composition (Pylkkänen et al, 2011), licensing of negative polarity items (Steinhauer et al, 2010; Panizza, 2012) and semantic subcategorization (Kuperberg et al, 2000). In our experiments we found that quantifiers which were pragmatically inconsistent with a context elicited a qualitatively different ERP response than quantifiers which were semantically inconsistent, suggesting that they were processed by different mechanisms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We predicted that highly-skilled performers would be relatively 173 immune to the effects of avoidant instructions because they have more conscious 174 attentional resources available to enable them to process the demonstrably complex 175 demands of this type of instruction (Janelle, 1999). Interestingly, recent cognitive 176 research (e.g., Panizza, 2012) shows that the comprehension of negative sentences or 177 instructions requires more attentional resources than does that of positive equivalents. 178 A likely explanation for this effect comes from the fact that the meaning of negated 179 instructions can be understood only after a cognitive representation of the positive 180 equivalent has been created.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…naturalistic methods apply quantification and statistics to phenomena that are intangible to measuring devices. in short, what the natural psychological sciences like neurosemantics share are attempts to represent consciousness and maintain natural psychological science (Panizza 2012, Pulvermuller 2012. the phenomenological point is to create an accurate narrative concerning how people experience verbal and nonverbal meaning.…”
Section: Summary: Psychology and Therapy Involve Philosophical Problementioning
confidence: 99%