1977
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000900000490
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Four early stages in the development of Li negation

Abstract: No reasonably successful theory of the acquisition of negation seems to have yet been proposed. Most studies describe post hoc what has taken place; but they fail to go on to suggest a theory that will predict what will take place. McNeill seems to be the only one to have explicitly aimed at a theory with at least a certain amount of predictive capacity, but his views leave much to be desired. This paper outlines an alternative proposal to cover four very early stages for the acquisition of negation systems in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

3
39
1
6

Year Published

1979
1979
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 85 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
3
39
1
6
Order By: Relevance
“…These action-verb cooccurrences could contribute, by means of Hebbian-like mechanisms, to the strength of the connections between the perisylvian language regions and the motor and premotor regions (Pulvermüller, 1999(Pulvermüller, , 2013Glenberg and Gallese, 2012). Moreover, in these early manipulative scenarios, adults frequently use negative imperatives (e.g., "don't touch that") to guide the child's behavior (Wode, 1977;Park, 1979;Austin et al, 2014). In this way, a child could learn the pragmatic function of negative imperatives as stop signals driving him or her to suppress the initiated or intended actions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These action-verb cooccurrences could contribute, by means of Hebbian-like mechanisms, to the strength of the connections between the perisylvian language regions and the motor and premotor regions (Pulvermüller, 1999(Pulvermüller, , 2013Glenberg and Gallese, 2012). Moreover, in these early manipulative scenarios, adults frequently use negative imperatives (e.g., "don't touch that") to guide the child's behavior (Wode, 1977;Park, 1979;Austin et al, 2014). In this way, a child could learn the pragmatic function of negative imperatives as stop signals driving him or her to suppress the initiated or intended actions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All languages have grammatical markers of negation and children understand and produce some negative statements from the second year of life (Wode, 1977;Austin et al, 2014). Behavioral studies indicate that negative sentences sometimes demand additional cognitive resources in comparison with affirmative sentences (Clark and Chase, 1972;Carpenter and Just, 1975;Kaup, 2006) and the presence of negation in a sentence immediately reduces the level of accessibility of the negated concept (MacDonald and Just, 1989;Kaup, 2001;Kaup and Zwaan, 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The study of negation in the first and second language acquisition of German has a much longer tradition (Becker 2005, Clahsen 1988, Dietrich & Grommes 1998, Meisel 1997, Verrips & Weissenborn 1992, Wode 1977 than the study of additive particles like auch (Benazzo 2003, Berger et al 2007, Dimroth 2002, Hulk 2003, Nederstigt 2003, Penner et al 2000, Schimke, Verhagen, and Dimroth 2008, Tracy 2002.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…
According to Wode (1977), the development of negation in children's speech goes through four stages.1 Stage I consists of one-word negation, i.e. children using a single nein, 'no', to express negation.
…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to Wode (1977), the development of negation in children's speech goes through four stages.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%