2013
DOI: 10.1017/s0272263112000885
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

From Seeing Adverbs to Seeing Verbal Morphology

Abstract: Adult learners have persistent difficulty processing second language (L2) inflectional morphology. We investigate associative learning explanations that involve the blocking of later experienced cues by earlier learned ones in the first language (L1; i.e., transfer) and the L2 (i.e., proficiency). Sagarra (2008) and Ellis and Sagarra (2010b) found that, unlike Spanish monolinguals, intermediate English-Spani… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
23
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
2
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Robenalt and Goldberg (2016) investigated L2 English users' judgments about the atypical transitive use of intransitive verbs, finding that L2 English users showed no evidence of taking alternative preempting formulations (e.g., Ashley disappeared/vanished) into account when judging atypical transitive sentences (e.g., The magician disappeared/vanished the rabbit), while English native speakers tended to judge novel sentences with a readily available alternative formulation to be less acceptable than novel sentences with no competing alternatives. In contrast, Ellis and colleagues (Ellis & Sagarra, 2011;Sagarra & Ellis, 2013) observed that salient competing cues (e.g., temporal adverbs) block L2 acquisition of less salient ones (e.g., verb inflections), which in part supports the preemption hypothesis. Further, Ellis, O'Donnell, and Römer (2013) found that advanced L2 speakers' verb argument construction use is sensitive to the verb contingency in verb argument constructions, verb-token frequency (entrenchment), and semantic prototypicality.…”
Section: Evidence For and Against The Three Mechanisms In L2 Developmentmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Robenalt and Goldberg (2016) investigated L2 English users' judgments about the atypical transitive use of intransitive verbs, finding that L2 English users showed no evidence of taking alternative preempting formulations (e.g., Ashley disappeared/vanished) into account when judging atypical transitive sentences (e.g., The magician disappeared/vanished the rabbit), while English native speakers tended to judge novel sentences with a readily available alternative formulation to be less acceptable than novel sentences with no competing alternatives. In contrast, Ellis and colleagues (Ellis & Sagarra, 2011;Sagarra & Ellis, 2013) observed that salient competing cues (e.g., temporal adverbs) block L2 acquisition of less salient ones (e.g., verb inflections), which in part supports the preemption hypothesis. Further, Ellis, O'Donnell, and Römer (2013) found that advanced L2 speakers' verb argument construction use is sensitive to the verb contingency in verb argument constructions, verb-token frequency (entrenchment), and semantic prototypicality.…”
Section: Evidence For and Against The Three Mechanisms In L2 Developmentmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…This has included presenting stimuli without temporal adverbs, thus forcing attention on the temporal meaning of verb inflections (e.g. Benati, 2005;Marsden, 2006;Marsden & Chen, 2011;Sagarra & Ellis, 2013), without overt subjects, thus forcing attention on person and number meanings (Marsden, 2006;Marsden et al 2013), or without lexical phrases for doubt and certainty, forcing attention on subjunctive versus indicative inflections (Fern ‡ndez, 2008). Task-essential form-meaning input mapping practice has been found to lead to more learning than input activities with equal numbers of Accepted for publication, Studies in Second Language Acquisition Kevin McManus (kmcmanus@psu.edu) 11 !…”
Section: Task-essential Form-meaning Mapping Practice For Aspectual Dmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…N. C. Ellis () attributed difficulties in noticing and acquiring inflectional morphology to what he called “learned attention” (also see Cintrón‐Valentín & N. C. Ellis, ; N. C. Ellis, ; N. C. Ellis & Sagarra, ; Sagarra & Ellis, ), an “associative learning” phenomenon whereby previously learned cues block those that are later encountered by the learners. In the current study, the future time frame was established in the model text through the multiple occurrences of the lexical item tomorrow, rendering the future tense marker redundant and unnecessary.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of the participants in the current study, 81.25% were L1 speakers of English, a morphologically poor language (Behney et al, ) compared to Arabic. Sagarra and N. C. Ellis () reported that learners with morphologically rich L1s (e.g., Romanian and Spanish) spent a longer time looking at verbs than those with a morphologically poor L1 (English). N. C. Ellis and Sagarra () remarked that adult learners in the early stages of SLA generally tend to focus on lexical items because they are physically salient in the input and psychologically salient due to the simplicity of their form‐function mapping.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation