2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.04.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Generalizing beyond the input: The functions of the constructions matter

Abstract: a b s t r a c tA growing emphasis on statistics in language learning raises the question of whether and when speakers use language in ways that go beyond the statistical regularities in the input. In this study, two groups were exposed to six novel verbs and two novel word order constructions that differed in function: one construction but not the other was exclusively used with pronoun undergoers. The distributional structure of the input was manipulated between groups according to whether each verb was used … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
48
2
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
1
48
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Implicit learning over these lexical-syntactic-event contingencies (Chang, Dell & Bock, 2006) should promote higher frequency event-lexio-syntactic choices over atypical mappings, including higher frequency lexico-syntactic pairings and event-syntactic pairings. Artificial language studies show that speakers learn event-structure mappings, with consequences for the particular construction used to describe that event (Perek & Goldberg, 2015;in press). Similarly, a natural language example might be seen in Korean, where events in which a patient is adversely affected are more associated with passive structure descriptions than events with non-adverse outcomes (Oshima, 2006;Park, 2005;Song & Choe, 2007).…”
Section: Multiple Forces Shaping Production Choicesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Implicit learning over these lexical-syntactic-event contingencies (Chang, Dell & Bock, 2006) should promote higher frequency event-lexio-syntactic choices over atypical mappings, including higher frequency lexico-syntactic pairings and event-syntactic pairings. Artificial language studies show that speakers learn event-structure mappings, with consequences for the particular construction used to describe that event (Perek & Goldberg, 2015;in press). Similarly, a natural language example might be seen in Korean, where events in which a patient is adversely affected are more associated with passive structure descriptions than events with non-adverse outcomes (Oshima, 2006;Park, 2005;Song & Choe, 2007).…”
Section: Multiple Forces Shaping Production Choicesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other evidence for fine-grained event-lexico-syntactic learning can be found in studies that suggest that the ease with which a speaker can capture the gist of an event has consequences for structure choice (Konopka & Meyer, 2014), and findings that show statistical learning between events and constructions. In artificial grammar learning studies, Perek and Goldberg (2015;in press) found that speakers learned not only lexico-syntactic patterns of the likelihood of certain verbs appearing in certain syntactic frames, but also learned event-syntactic patterns in the likelihood of certain syntactic forms describing certain event types, and applied this knowledge when using the novel language to describe pictures. The mapping between adverse events and passive sentence structures is a clear natural language example of these same phenomena.…”
Section: Implications For Lexical Syntactic and Event Knowledge In mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Overgeneralizations in early child language have attracted a lot of attention in the field of language acquisition (Albright & Hayes, 2003;Ambridge, Freudenthal, Pine, Mills, Clark, & Rowland, 2009;Ambridge, 2010;Ambridge, Pine, Rowland, Chang, & Bidgood, 2013;Anđel, Klampfer, Kilani-Schoch, Dressler, & Kovačević, 2000;Bowerman, 1988;Brooks, Tomasello, Dodson, & Lewis, 1999;Brown, 1973;Katičić, 2003;Kuczaj, 1977;Hržica, 2012;Li & MacWhinney, 1996;MacWhinney, 1976;MacWhinney, 1993;Marcus, Pinker, Ullman, Hollander, Rosen, & Xu, 1992;Slobin, 1973;Perek & Goldberg, 2015;Theakston, 2004). In the domain of verbal morphological overgeneralizations, the typically discussed cases are overregularization errors of the English Past Tense -ed and the German regular -t participle, with the overregularization rate of 4% to 5% (Clahsen & Rothweiler, 1993;Markus et al, 1992).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%