IntroductionMice (Mus musculus domesticus) produce ultrasonic vocalizations in several behavioral contexts. Pups emit an ultrasonic isolation call when they are separated from their mother and littermates [1,2] and adult males produce ultrasonic vocalizations with sequential complexity comparable to birdsong when they encounter females [3][4][5][6].In the genus Mus, several subspecies have been used to create laboratory mouse strains. Previously, one of the authors (Y.M.) established laboratory mice derived from M. m. molossinus, Japanese wild-derived inbred mice, which are named KOR1, KOR5, KOR7, AIZ2, and MAE [7]. Although mice seldom emit vocalizations in an audible range, approximately 10% of male KOR1 mice and about 2% of female KOR1 mice produce successive, songlike audible vocalizations after 3 months of age [8]. Previous studies revealed that M. m. domesticus produce audible vocalizations when they feel pain or disgust [6,9]. However, these audible vocalizations by M. m. domesticus are not emitted in a successive train as in KOR1 [6,9]. Therefore, it is not clear whether the successive audible vocalizations by KOR1 and by M. m. domesticus are produced in the same behavioral contexts. In addition, if the vocalization is produced in the same contexts by both subspecies, it is possible that KOR1 will also produce ultrasonic vocalizations during adult courtship and when a pup is isolated.In this study, we 1) sought behavioral contexts that induce successive audible vocalizations in KOR1 and 2) examined whether KOR1 emits ultrasonic vocalizations in the contexts in which they have been observed in other subspecies. Specifically, we tested the vocal responses in both audible and ultrasonic ranges using adult and infant KOR1 mice in several contexts that arouse distress, anxiety, and sexual motivation.