1977
DOI: 10.1016/0031-9384(77)90224-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Genetic and environmental variability in lick rates of mice

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
19
0

Year Published

1986
1986
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
5
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Early on, control and SOD1-G93A mice exhibited lick frequency and tongue force values well within the range of published values (Horowitz et al, 1977;Murakami, 1977;Wang and Fowler, 1999). As the disease progressed, SOD1-G93A mice exhibited a tongue motility deficit by 100 days of age, followed by a tongue force deficit that became apparent by 115 days of age.…”
Section: Orolingual Motor Deficitssupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Early on, control and SOD1-G93A mice exhibited lick frequency and tongue force values well within the range of published values (Horowitz et al, 1977;Murakami, 1977;Wang and Fowler, 1999). As the disease progressed, SOD1-G93A mice exhibited a tongue motility deficit by 100 days of age, followed by a tongue force deficit that became apparent by 115 days of age.…”
Section: Orolingual Motor Deficitssupporting
confidence: 75%
“…This rate of licking is relatively resistant to changes in motivational state or stimulus, leading to speculation that the rate of licking is controlled by a CPG. We [7] and others [28, 30] previously reported that D2 mice have a faster rate of licking than B6 mice (a difference in MPI of 24 ms in our earlier work). In the current sample of 18 strains, MPI showed a similar range of values as in our study of BXD RI strains [8].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…With respect to fluid licking and microstructure, mice of the commonly used strain C57BL/6J have been shown in several studies to possess slower lick rates (longer mean interlick intervals) to water or other tastant solutions than DBA/2, 129P3/J, or SWR/J mice [7, 2831]. Further strain differences have been shown in other microstructural parameters including overall consumption, burst count, burst size and initial licking rate [7, 31, 32].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As mentioned earlier, experimental manipulations such as sensory deafferentation, increased thirst, or using different taste stimuli rarely result in any change in the inherent lick frequency of mice, which is determined by a central pattern generator (Horowitz, G. P. et al 1977;Boughter, J. D., Jr. et al 2007b;Shires, C. B. et al 2011). B6 mice possess a slower primary lick rate, as reflected by an average MPI around 120 ms, whereas D2 are faster lickers, with an average MPI of about 95 ms (Boughter, J. D., Jr. et al 2007b).…”
Section: Lick Ratementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another measure we examined in our study was the intra-burst lick rate, defined as the average ILI < 1 s. It is important to distinguish this measure from the primary lick rate (MPI -mean primary interval), which is defined as the mean ILI between 60 and 160 ms (Boughter, J. D., Jr. et al 2007b). MPI is genetically determined among strains of mice, is thought to reflect a "hard-wired" output of an intrinsic pattern generator, and has been shown to be impervious to sensory or environmental manipulation (Horowitz, G. P. et al 1977;Boughter, J. D., Jr. et al 2007b;Shires, C. B. et al 2011). On the other hand, the intra-burst lick rate includes longer duration ILIs, and is affected by gastric feedback.…”
Section: Microstructurementioning
confidence: 99%