2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.05.026
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Giant Australian cuttlefish use mutual assessment to resolve male-male contests

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
25
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
2
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For this reason, although some species may settle their contests based on mutual assessment (e.g. Junior & Peixoto, ; Schnell et al , ; Green & Patek, ), our results for the relationships between contest duration and traits linked to RHP suggest that the proportion of species following mutual assessment is low and, therefore, self‐assessment seems to be the most frequent contest resolution rule adopted by species.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 71%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For this reason, although some species may settle their contests based on mutual assessment (e.g. Junior & Peixoto, ; Schnell et al , ; Green & Patek, ), our results for the relationships between contest duration and traits linked to RHP suggest that the proportion of species following mutual assessment is low and, therefore, self‐assessment seems to be the most frequent contest resolution rule adopted by species.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…It is intriguing that SAM is not a general contest resolution rule for species that signal their RHP (Jennings et al , ; Briffa, ; Painting & Holwell, ; Schnell et al, ), species that have high energetic expenditure during the fight (e.g. Copeland et al , ) or species that fight with high chances of injury (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A recent study on the giant Australian cuttlefish S. apama reported similar structural patterns during male-male contests. Male S. apama matched behaviors during low-level aggression; however, winners outperformed losers during high-level aggression (Schnell et al 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…For example, many Cervidae stags fight for the control of the females (Clutton-Brock et al 1979;McElligott et al 1998); such activity is common in mammals (Packer 2001;Casey et al 2015). Fighting is also extremely common in invertebrate species such as cephalopods (Schnell et al 2015) and weaponless butterflies (Kemp and Wiklund 2001). Hymenopterans, such as ants, wasps, and bees, are well-known to engage in intraspecific fighting, either to protect their colonies (Gloag et al 2008;Rudolph and McEntee 2015) or to compete for the queen position (Berthelot et al 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%