2019
DOI: 10.1017/s0025100319000045
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Glottalisation of word-final stops in Australian English unstressed syllables

Abstract: Glottalisation functions as a cue to coda stop voicelessness in many varieties of English, occurring most commonly for alveolar stops, although varieties differ according to the context and frequency with which glottalisation is used. In Australian English, younger speakers glottalise voiceless coda stops at much higher rates than older speakers suggesting a recent change to the variety, yet this change has only been examined in stressed syllables for stops with alveolar place of articulation. In addition, res… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results help address the question posed by Passy (1900) cited at the beginning of our paper: we find little evidence that voiceless [h] and voiced [ç] are distinct -at least in terms of presence and strength of voicing -between two vowels and in final postvocalic position. This is illustrated in Figure 11, which shows two tokens (one initial, the other intervocalic) of voiceless [h] and voiced [ç] in two related languages: Upper Sorbian (which has /h/) and Czech (which has /ç/).…”
Section: Glottal Fricatives and Breathy Vowelssupporting
confidence: 57%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These results help address the question posed by Passy (1900) cited at the beginning of our paper: we find little evidence that voiceless [h] and voiced [ç] are distinct -at least in terms of presence and strength of voicing -between two vowels and in final postvocalic position. This is illustrated in Figure 11, which shows two tokens (one initial, the other intervocalic) of voiceless [h] and voiced [ç] in two related languages: Upper Sorbian (which has /h/) and Czech (which has /ç/).…”
Section: Glottal Fricatives and Breathy Vowelssupporting
confidence: 57%
“…For about as long as the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) has existed, phoneticians (e.g. Meyer 1900, Passy 1900) have acknowledged that glottal consonants show large variation in voicing. And when glottal consonants are voiced, they look very similar to nonmodal (breathy and creaky) vowels.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is interesting that the deployment of glottalisation is becoming common in AusE in a number of contexts including the implementation of coda voicelessness (Penney et al 2018(Penney et al , 2020(Penney et al , 2021, the use of creaky voice quality (Dallaston & Docherty 2019, White et al 2021, and in hiatus management (Cox et al 2014b;Yuen et al 2017Yuen et al , 2018. A fruitful area for future research could be to explore the intriguing relationship between these various segmental, sociophonetic, and prosodic uses for glottalisation in the phonological toolkit.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the word 'bat' may be pronounced as [baet], [baet], [baeʔt], or [baeʔ]. Voiceless stop glottalization is common across languages (Harris, 2001;Kohler, 1994;Michaud, 2004), though in English it is better documented outside North America, where it has more widespread socio-indexical meaning (Ashby & Przedlacka, 2014;Clark & Watson, 2016;Docherty & Foulkes, 1999a;Docherty, Hay, & Walker, 2006;Fabricius, 2002;Gordeeva & Scobbie, 2013;Henton & Bladon, 1988;Higginbottom, 1964;Holmes, 1995;Johnston, 2007;Kerswill, 2007;Mathisen, 1999;Mees & Collins, 1999;Milroy, Milroy, Hartley, & Walshaw, 1994;Newbrook, 1999;Penney, Cox, Miles, & Palethorpe, 2018;Penney, Cox, & Szakay, 2019;Ramisch, 2007;Roach, 1973Roach, , 1979Stuart-Smith, 1999;Tollfree, 1999Tollfree, , 2001Watt & Milroy, 1999;Williams & Kerswill, 1999).…”
Section: American English Coda Glottalizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In several varieties of American English, coda glottalization is reportedly more common for younger speakers (Eddington & Channer, 2010;Eddington & Taylor, 2009;Roberts, 2006), and Kaźmierski (2020) finds the same qualitative pattern for prevocalic word-final /t/ glottalization in the Buckeye Corpus. In some English varieties outside the United States, younger speakers also have higher rates of glottal stop production (Holmes, 1995;Mathisen, 1999Penney et al, 2019Smith & Holmes-Elliott, 2018;Stoddart et al, 1999;Watt & Milroy, 1999).…”
Section: Age and Gender Of The Speakermentioning
confidence: 99%