2002
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.919
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Guilty by mere association: Evaluative conditioning and the spreading attitude effect.

Abstract: Five experiments investigated the phenomenon that attitude formation is not confined to the co-occurrence of an attitudinal object with an evaluated experience. The pairing of a target with a (dis)liked person not only affects the evaluation of the previously neutral person but spreads to other individuals who are (pre)associated with the target (spreading attitude effect). Experiments 1 and 2 provided evidence for the spreading attitude effect in appetitive as well as aversive evaluative conditioning. Experim… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
253
6
8

Year Published

2003
2003
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 217 publications
(278 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
11
253
6
8
Order By: Relevance
“…seen as the prototypical case of EC, but it also imposes a priori restrictions on its properties. For example, if EC is limited to changes in liking of neutral stimuli that are paired with valenced stimuli, phenomena such as sensory pre-conditioning (i.e., a change in liking of CS1 after experiencing CS1-CS2 trials that are followed by CS2-US trials; e.g., Hammerl & Grabitz, 1996;Walther, 2002) would not qualify as instances of EC. A further difference between the definition by De Houwer and some other definitions is that it does not refer to the direction in which liking changes.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…seen as the prototypical case of EC, but it also imposes a priori restrictions on its properties. For example, if EC is limited to changes in liking of neutral stimuli that are paired with valenced stimuli, phenomena such as sensory pre-conditioning (i.e., a change in liking of CS1 after experiencing CS1-CS2 trials that are followed by CS2-US trials; e.g., Hammerl & Grabitz, 1996;Walther, 2002) would not qualify as instances of EC. A further difference between the definition by De Houwer and some other definitions is that it does not refer to the direction in which liking changes.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on the influence of secondary tasks during the presentation of CS-US pairings is relevant for the resource-independence of the processes that operate during the INTRODUCTION 21 acquisition phase (e.g., Dedonder et al, 2010;Pleyers et al, 2009;Walther, 2002). It has to be noted, however, that potential effects of a secondary task could also be due to differences in task-related goals.…”
Section: Resource Independencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The present research has several advances over previous work on implicit balance. The most important of these extensions has to do with moving from static to more dynamic views of implicit balance in consumer persuasion (Gawronski, Walther, & Blank, 2005;Langer et al, 2009;Walther, 2002). That is, whereas most previous research has focused on the examination of implicitly measured constructs (e.g., the self, an object or a group, and valence), the present research took a more experimental approach to examine implicit balance.…”
Section: Implications For Implicit Balancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…This case implies a change in implicit but not explicit attitudes, with the two being generally uncorrelated. According to the APE model, such patterns should emerge when (a) a given factor leads to a change in pattern activation or associative structure and, additionally, (b) other relevant propositions lead to a rejection of associative evaluations as a valid basis for an evaluative judgment.An illustrative example for this pattern is again found in research on EC, which has shown that conditioning effects on evaluative judgments are often reduced when participants become aware of the contingency between CS und US Fulcher & Hammerl, 2001;Walther, 2002). From the perspective of the APE model, one could argue that contingency awareness reduces participants' trust in the validity of their associative evaluations, such that actual changes in associative structure are not reflected in evaluative judgments.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%