2000
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.53
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior.

Abstract: This study tested the idea of habits as a form of goal-directed automatic behavior. Expanding on the idea that habits are mentally represented as associations between goals and actions, it was proposed that goals are capable of activating the habitual action. More specific, when habits are established (e.g., frequent cycling to the university), the very activation of the goal to act (e.g., having to attend lectures at the university) automatically evokes the habitual response (e.g., bicycle). Indeed, it was te… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

26
696
4
8

Year Published

2000
2000
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 969 publications
(764 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
26
696
4
8
Order By: Relevance
“…As cognitive entities, goals are assumed to be associatively linked to other cognitive entities, and in particular, to actions perceived as instrumental to their attainment, that is, to means assumed to lead to those particular goals (see also, Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000Bargh & Chartrand, 1999;Bargh, Gollwitzer, LeeChai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001;Moskowitz, Gollwitzer, Wasel, & Schaal, 1999). A high level goal may be connected to lower level sub-goals, in turn connected to their own attainment means as well as to other possible sub-goals.…”
Section: Goal Systems Theory: Affective Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As cognitive entities, goals are assumed to be associatively linked to other cognitive entities, and in particular, to actions perceived as instrumental to their attainment, that is, to means assumed to lead to those particular goals (see also, Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000Bargh & Chartrand, 1999;Bargh, Gollwitzer, LeeChai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001;Moskowitz, Gollwitzer, Wasel, & Schaal, 1999). A high level goal may be connected to lower level sub-goals, in turn connected to their own attainment means as well as to other possible sub-goals.…”
Section: Goal Systems Theory: Affective Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given that goals can influence a person's intentions through automatic processes (e.g., Aarts and Dijksterhuis 2000;Sheeran et al 2005a), which show little to no decline with aging (e.g., Rogers 2000), a reasonable prediction would be that these effects should be similar for the two age groups. Therefore, our tentative prediction was that both young and older adults would rate their goal-related prospective memory tasks (and concern-related prospective memory tasks) as more important than other tasks.…”
Section: Predictions For Young Versus Older Adultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on theory of divided attention and multitasking (as described in Sect. 2.1), actions may shift between periphery and center of attention as a result of various factors, such as difficulty [13] of the operation or habituation [14,15] of the activity. These factors may clearly differ from person to person: Habituation happens only if individuals gain experience in performing an activity, and certain activities may simply be more difficult for one person than for another.…”
Section: Peripheral Interaction's Personal Naturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The resource demand of activities depends on several factors such as the difficulty of the operation. Additionally, the automaticity [13] or habituation [14,15] of activities influences the amount of resources required: activities that have been trained extensively, such as walking, require only few mental resources. The division of resources over activities is furthermore influenced by the likelihood of activities being performed, which is managed by the supervisory attentional system [16].…”
Section: Divided Attention and Multitasking Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation