“…Given that there is no inherent visual connotation in the Latin root of the word, it is perhaps no surprise that ‘vection’ is now increasingly being used to also refer to illusory self-motions induced by stimulating the non-visual self-motion senses . These non-visual illusions of self-motion (where the observer is typically either seated in darkness or blindfolded) include: (1) Auditory vection – illusory self-motion induced by moving the observer’s acoustic surround ( Dodge, 1923 ; Lackner, 1977 ; Sakamoto et al, 2004 ; Riecke et al, 2008 ; Keshavarz et al, 2014 ; see Väljamäe, 2009 for a review); (2) Haptokinetic vection – illusory self-motion produced by applying tactile motion stimulation to large areas of the observer’s body ( Dichgans and Brandt, 1978 ; Nilsson et al, 2012 ; Nordahl et al, 2012 ; Murata et al, 2014 ); (3) Arthrokinetic vection – illusory self-motion induced by passively rotating the observer’s limb/s ( Brandt et al, 1977 ; Howard et al, 1998 ); and (4) Biomechanical vection – illusory self-motion generated when a standing/seated subject repeatedly steps on a treadmill ( Bles, 1981 ; Riecke et al, 2011 ). Interestingly, while illusions of self-motion can also be induced by caloric (e.g., Fasold et al, 2002 ) and direct galvanic stimulation (e.g., Cress et al, 1997 ; Lepecq et al, 2006 ), such vestibular illusions are rarely referred to as vestibular vection (see below for one notable exception) 2 .…”