2020
DOI: 10.1017/9781108616522
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How to Study Animal Minds

Abstract: Comparative psychology, the multidisciplinary study of animal behavior and psychology, confronts the challenge of how to study animals we find cute and easy to anthropomorphize, and animals we find odd and easy to objectify, without letting these biases negatively impact the science. In this Element, Kristin Andrews identifies and critically examines the principles of comparative psychology and shows how they can introduce other biases by objectifying animal subjects and encouraging scientists to remain detach… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
35
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 150 publications
0
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Over 70% of our sample somewhat or strongly agreed that Morgan’s canon is important to use when interpreting the results of animal cognition experiments. Superficially, this contrasts with a large body of literature criticising the canon on the grounds that there is no reason to privilege “simpler” or “lower” explanations of animal cognition over more “complicated” or “higher” explanations [ 20 , 21 , 55 60 ]. However, participants qualitative responses revealed a more nuanced picture: Many of those who also provided free-text responses, a) recognised the inherent ambiguity and multiple interpretations of Morgan’s canon, and, b) cautioned against a blind application of Morgan’s canon.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Over 70% of our sample somewhat or strongly agreed that Morgan’s canon is important to use when interpreting the results of animal cognition experiments. Superficially, this contrasts with a large body of literature criticising the canon on the grounds that there is no reason to privilege “simpler” or “lower” explanations of animal cognition over more “complicated” or “higher” explanations [ 20 , 21 , 55 60 ]. However, participants qualitative responses revealed a more nuanced picture: Many of those who also provided free-text responses, a) recognised the inherent ambiguity and multiple interpretations of Morgan’s canon, and, b) cautioned against a blind application of Morgan’s canon.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…This is a possible factor underlying poor performance and inconclusive evidence in many studies with non-primate species. Tasks optimally designed from a human viewpoint may not adequately consider the ecology and ways in which other animals perceive and interact with the world (Andrews, 2020; e.g., elephants use scent in quantity discrimination, Plotnik et al, 2019), thus not accurately testing cognitive processes. Another layer of complexity is added by idiosyncratic methods that emerge with each adaptation to a new species, making it difficult to evaluate the reason that different species succeed or fail on tasks lacking clear comparison points.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Over 70% of our sample somewhat or strongly agreed that Morgan's canon is important to use when interpreting the results of animal cognition experiments. Superficially, this contrasts with a large body of literature criticising the canon on the grounds that there is no reason to privilege "simpler" or "lower" explanations of animal cognition over more "complicated" or "higher" explanations (Andrews, 2020;Bausman & Halina, 2018;Buckner, 2013;Fitzpatrick, 2008;Heyes, 2012;Meketa, 2014;Sober, 2005;Starzak, 2017). However, participants qualitative responses revealed a more nuanced picture: Many of those who also provided free-text responses, a) recognised the inherent ambiguity and multiple interpretations of Morgan's canon, and b) cautioned against a blind application of Morgan's canon.…”
Section: Morgan's Canonmentioning
confidence: 91%