2018
DOI: 10.1364/josaa.35.00b256
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hyperspectral database of fruits and vegetables

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
27
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
1
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Hyperspectral images were taken under the same conditions as the photographs used in the experiments, although they appeared less sharp, probably because of the limitations of the optics of the hyperspectral camera. Our hyperspectral measuring system is described in detail in our previous work (Ennis, Schiller, Toscani, & Gegenfurtner, 2018). Analyses of the hyperspectral images confirmed the results from the photographs.…”
Section: Stimuli and Apparatussupporting
confidence: 63%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Hyperspectral images were taken under the same conditions as the photographs used in the experiments, although they appeared less sharp, probably because of the limitations of the optics of the hyperspectral camera. Our hyperspectral measuring system is described in detail in our previous work (Ennis, Schiller, Toscani, & Gegenfurtner, 2018). Analyses of the hyperspectral images confirmed the results from the photographs.…”
Section: Stimuli and Apparatussupporting
confidence: 63%
“…For example, when judging the lightness of a texture pattern, observers based their matches either on the lightest or the darkest areas, depending on contrast relative to the background ( Toscani, Valsecchi, & Gegenfurtner, 2013b ). Furthermore, although color matching experiments had shown that observers tend to base their matches on the most saturated parts of the targets’ color distributions, when participants are asked to classify a large sample of photographs of leaves they based their judgments on the average chromaticity of the leaves’ color distributions ( Milojevic, Ennis, Toscani, & Gegenfurtner, 2018 ). Our results provide additional evidence that color matching is based on different parts of the targets’ color distributions according to the task demands.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, there are additional datasets of measured surface reflectances that could be incorporated into future analyses. Some of these datasets focused on the reflectance of objects (e.g., fruit) that are thought to be important for the evolution of primate color vision (e.g., Sumner & Mollon, 2000; Regan et al, 2001; Barnard et al, 2002; Ennis, Schiller, Toscani, & Gegenfurtner, 2018). Another issue, not addressed by these datasets, is relative frequency of different surface reflectances in natural viewing.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In case fresh fruit and vegetables, or controllable multiprimary LEDs, are unavailable, the neon fruit illusion may also be simulated. This would not be possible with standard three-channel photographs, but a good approximation can be achieved using a recently published hyperspectral database of fruits and vegetables (Ennis, Schiller, Toscani, & Gegenfurtner, 2018). The results of a lime and a red pepper, illuminated by a narrowband green LED with and without the minor contribution of a narrowband red LED, are shown in Figure 4.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A simulation of the neon fruit illusion, using hyperspectral images of a lime and a red pepper from a database of hyperspectral images of fruits and vegetables (Ennis et al., 2018). We estimated reflectance of the lime and red pepper by dividing the spectrum of reflected light reported in the database by a measurement of the illuminant spectrum at a pixel location free from obvious specular highlights.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%