“…Many dimensions of speaker-specific behaviour relate to linguistic structure and linguistic categories, but in ways that cannot be captured if speech is considered solely in terms of an inventory of phonemes and major intonational categories� Rather, there are dimensions of speaker-specific behaviour that involve phonetic detail� As defined by (among others) Local (2003) and Hawkins (Hawkins and Smith, 2001;Hawkins, 2003Hawkins, , 2010, phonetic detail refers to phonetic information that affects people's responses but "is not considered a major, usually local, perceptual cue for phonemic contrasts in the citation forms of lexical items" (Hawkins and Local, 2007: 181)� This type of information is "systematically distributed [according to linguistic/communicative function] but not systematically treated in conventional approaches" (ibid. )� Thus, phonetic detail refers not to information that mainly distinguishes phonemes (such as /pa/ vs� /ba/), but to cues that distinguish other aspects of linguistic structure, such as prosodic structure (compare the unstressed /p/ in potato with the stressed /p/ in important); syllabic and morphological structure (/p/ is more heavily aspirated in the morphologically-complex word displease than in the mono-morphemic word displays; Smith et al�, 2012); or pragmatic function (for Standard Southern British English, both [pʰ] and [p'] are possible allophones of /p/ in it's a tap, but the ejective sounds more emphatic, definite, and final than the aspirated stop� The range of aspects of linguistic structure that condition systematic variation in phonetic detail is extensive� Crucially for the present purposes, there is evidence of speaker-specific variation in many of them, henceforth termed speaker-specific phonetic detail (SSPD)� For example, speakers vary in the extent to which they coarticulate, and in the precise coarticulatory strategies that they use� Reviewing research in this area, Kühnert and Nolan (1999) comment that it is relatively scarce, and that "the high variability found in the data makes it difficult to distinguish between effects which should be considered as being idiosyncratic and effects which simply reflect the allowed range of variation for the phenomenon"� Nonetheless, they identify several experiments showing individual coarticulatory differences: among British English speakers in coarticulation of /r/ and /l/ with a following vowel (Nolan, 1983(Nolan, , 1985, and among both Swedish (Lubker and Gay, 1982) and English speakers (Perkell and Matthies, 1992) in the timing of movements for anticipatory lip rounding� Some of this variation may be due to an individual's genetic (anatomical and physiological) inheritance, as suggested by Weirich et al�'s (2013) finding that tongue looping trajectories are more similar in monozygotic twins than in dizygotic twins or unrelated speakers (though see Nolan and Oh, 1996 for a demonstration of articulatory variability within identical twin pairs)� Speakers also vary in their "prosodic signatures", i� e� the detailed phonetic means they use to index prosodic prominence and prosodic bound-aries� With respect to prominence, individual speakers mark prominent as opposed to non-promi...…”