1996
DOI: 10.1007/bf02227965
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Implicit memory bias for threat in posttraumatic stress disorder

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

5
62
1
3

Year Published

2002
2002
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 98 publications
(71 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
5
62
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Although the results were signi¢cant only at one noise level, Amir et al (1996) found that panic patients exhibited enhanced priming for threat sentences than for neutral sentences, whereas control subjects exhibited the opposite pattern. Very similar results occurred in a study on Vietnam combat veterans with and without PTSD (Amir et al 1996b).…”
Section: E Mot Ion -C Ong Ru E N T M E Mory Bi a Ssupporting
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although the results were signi¢cant only at one noise level, Amir et al (1996) found that panic patients exhibited enhanced priming for threat sentences than for neutral sentences, whereas control subjects exhibited the opposite pattern. Very similar results occurred in a study on Vietnam combat veterans with and without PTSD (Amir et al 1996b).…”
Section: E Mot Ion -C Ong Ru E N T M E Mory Bi a Ssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…For example, psychopathologists have adapted the`white noise' paradigm (Jacoby et al 1988) to test for emotion-congruent implicit memory biases in panic disorder (Amir et al 1996a) and PTSD (Amir et al 1996b). In the ¢rst study, panic patients and healthy control subjects heard threat (e.g.`The anxious woman panicked in the supermarket') and neutral (e.g.`The shiny apple sat on the table') sentences.…”
Section: E Mot Ion -C Ong Ru E N T M E Mory Bi a Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Vietnam veterans with PTSD exhibited an enhancement of implicit memory (i.e. recall following priming by words stems) for combat words, relative to neutral words in comparison subjects (Zeitlin and McNally, 1991;Amir et al, 1996). Furthermore, in emotional Stroop-tasks, where participants have to name the color of words that are presented to them, patients with PTSD take significantly longer to name the color of trauma-related words (i.e.…”
Section: Automatic Processing Of Trauma-related Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Women with a history of CSA often display unusually high levels of attention for trauma or threat-related information (Amir, McNally, & Wiegartz, 1996), and show memory impairments that are not attributable to nontrauma factors, such as IQ or education levels (Bremner et al, 1995;Gray & Lombardo, 2001). Sexually abused women who develop posttraumatic stress disorder allocate more cognitive resources toward threatening stimuli than non-threatening stimuli (e.g., Foa, Feske, Murdock, & Kozak, 1991).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on research indicating that sexually abused women tend to link negative affect to sexual stimuli , we hypothesized that sexually relevant texts written by sexually abused women would show a higher frequency of negative emotions and a lower frequency of positive emotions as compared to texts written by women with no history of abuse. Additionally, based on studies that found an over-preoccupation with trauma-related behaviors (e.g., Amir et al, 1996), and based on Herman's (1992) model on trauma and recovery, the sexual desire function of sexually abused women was expected to be linked to a higher frequency of sexual and body words used when writing sexually relevant texts. An additional aim of this study was to establish whether the language utilized in sexually-relevant texts was linked to levels of function in sexual desire.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%