2018
DOI: 10.1177/1368430218773403
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Individual differences in system justification predict power and morality-related needs in advantaged and disadvantaged groups in response to group disparity

Abstract: Guided by the needs-based model, we explored how individual differences in system justification predict group members' needs in response to information about group-based disparities. Across two studies (N = 819), we found that among disadvantaged-group members (LGBTIQ* individuals/women) system justification was negatively related to need for power. Among advantaged-group members ([cis-]heterosexuals/men), system justification was negatively related to motivation to restore their ingroup's moral essence (i.e.,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
35
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
3
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The latter result is consistent with Hässler et al . 's () finding that members of the LGBTQ community had a higher need for morality than cis‐heterosexuals, possibly because they internalized the stigma portraying sexual and gender minority members as morally deviant (Herek & McLemore, ). A similar process of stigma internalization may have occurred among Israeli Arabs participants in Study 1 (see Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, , for the finding that Muslim Israelis located their outgroup, Israeli Jews, as higher on the moral–social dimension than their own ingroup).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The latter result is consistent with Hässler et al . 's () finding that members of the LGBTQ community had a higher need for morality than cis‐heterosexuals, possibly because they internalized the stigma portraying sexual and gender minority members as morally deviant (Herek & McLemore, ). A similar process of stigma internalization may have occurred among Israeli Arabs participants in Study 1 (see Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, , for the finding that Muslim Israelis located their outgroup, Israeli Jews, as higher on the moral–social dimension than their own ingroup).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Also, on the basis of findings that the need for morality could reflect the wish to either protect the ingroup's moral essence (i.e., core moral values) or defend its moral reputation (Allpress, Brown, Giner‐Sorolla, Deonna, & Teroni, ), we included two additional measures. Adapted from Hässler, Shnabel, Ullrich, Arditti‐Vogel, and SimanTow‐Nachlieli (), three items measured the wish that the ingroup behave more morally, reflecting the need for moral essence (e.g., ‘In order to give the Arabs equal treatment we, Jews, should be ready to pay a certain price if needed’); α = .89. Three additional items measured the need to defend the ingroup's moral reputation (e.g., ‘I would like the Arabs in Israel to acknowledge that they receive fair treatment from the Jews’); α = .96.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…System justification theory, as I conceive of it, is highly ‘practical' or ‘relevant' in the Lewinian sense that it is useful for diagnosing and addressing social problems, including many problems that apologists for the status quo would prefer to ignore. These include racism, colorism, sexism, classism, self‐objectification, tolerance of corruption, legitimation of social and economic inequality, hostility towards immigrants, scepticism about climate change, and acceptance of environmentally harmful industrial practices, among many other things (e.g., Brescoll et al ., ; Calogero & Jost, ; Chapleau & Oswald, ; Choma & Prusaczyk, ; Feygina et al ., ; García‐Sánchez et al ., ; Hässler et al ., ; Hennes et al ., ; Intawan & Nicholson, ; Jost, ; Jost & Kay, ; Kay & Jost, ; Napier & Jost, ; Napier et al ., ; Pacilli et al ., ; Shepherd & Kay, ; Tan, Liu, Huang, Zheng, & Liang, ; Vainio, Mäkiniemi, & Paloniemi, ; van der Toorn et al ., , ). Throughout this article I have sought to provide examples of the ways in which system justification theory can be applied to better understand societal phenomena.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…System justification not only decreases negative affect and increases satisfaction with the status quo, it diminishes support for system‐challenging protest activity (Jost, Becker, Osborne, & Badaan, ; Jost et al ., ) and the ‘will to power' among members of disadvantaged groups (Hässler, Shnabel, Ullrich, Arditti‐Vogel, & SimanTov‐Nachlieli, ). For instance, an experiment conducted in Germany revealed that when young women were exposed to relatively subtle, ‘benevolent' justifications for sexism, they subsequently expressed more positive affect, scored higher on gender‐specific system justification, and were less willing to participate in collective action on behalf of women (Becker & Wright, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, how the strength of the motive ebbs and flows based on contextual factors that heighten or diminish it in that moment. In this way, the system justification motive operates like many other types of goals (Jost et al ., ; Liviatan & Jost, , ) and is, in a sense, a multifinal subgoal (Kruglanski et al ., ) that satisfies a broad constellation of other psychologically important needs (Hässler, Shnabel, Ullrich, Arditti‐Vogel, & SimanTov‐Nachlieli, ; Hennes, Nam, Stern, & Jost, ; Jost, Becker, Osborne, & Badaan, ; Vargas‐Salfate, Paez, Khan, Liu, & Gil de Zúñiga, ). These include epistemic needs to see the world as consistent, structured, and orderly (Federico, Ergun, & Hunt, ; Jost & Krochik, ; Kay, Whitson, Gaucher, & Galinsky, ), existential needs to reduce threat and anxiety (Jost et al ., ), and relational needs to see the world in the same way that others do (Jost, Ledgerwood, & Hardin, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%