“…More recently, a few studies have applied online measures, such as eye‐tracking and event‐related brain potentials (ERPs), to investigate how readers keep track of temporal and emotional shifts in stories, and have demonstrated that readers are sensitive to mismatches between a character's expected and described emotional states [Carminati & Knoeferle, 2013, 2016; Komeda & Kusumi, 2006; Leuthold, Filik, Murphy, & Mackenzie, 2012; Munster, Carminati, & Knoeferle, 2014; Ralph‐Nearman & Filik, 2018; Rinck & Bower, 2000; Vega, 1996; Zwaan, 1996]. Moreover, some researchers have examined the online processes underlying sarcasm comprehension using eye‐tracking [e.g., Au‐Yeung, Kaakinen, Liversedge, & Benson, 2015; Deliens, Antoniou, Clin, Ostashchenko, & Kissine, 2018; Filik, Howman, Ralph‐Nearman, & Giora, 2018; Filik, Leuthold, Wallington, & Page, 2014; Filik & Moxey, 2010; Kaakinen, Olkoniemi, Kinnari, & Hyönä, 2014; Olkoniemi, Ranta, & Kaakinen, 2016; Olkoniemi, Johander, & Kaakinen, 2019; Olkoniemi, Strömberg, & Kaakinen, 2019; Țurcan & Filik, 2016, 2017]. These studies generally find that comprehending irony incurs higher processing costs than comprehending literal language, suggesting that the salient meaning (i.e., the most familiar, frequent, and conventional meaning) is activated by default and must be overridden to interpret ironic statements, irrespective of how biasing the context is [Giora, 1997, 2003].…”