2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.11.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inducing circular vection with tactile stimulation encircling the waist

Abstract: In general, moving sensory stimuli (visual and auditory) can induce illusory sensations of self-motion (i.e. vection) in the direction opposite of the sensory stimulation. The aim of the current study was to examine whether tactile stimulation encircling the waist could induce circular vection (around the body's yaw axis) and to examine whether this type of stimulation would influence participants' walking trajectory and balance. We assessed the strength and direction of perceived self-motion while vision was … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
36
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
36
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the 23 articles we found through our literature search, the application of tactile stimuli varied from applying forces to the body (Oishi et al, 2016), blowing wind (Churan et al, 2017;Feng et al, 2016;Seno, Ogawa, et al, 2011), stretching the skin (Horie et al, 2018) to vibro-tactile stimulation of various body parts. Vibro-tactile stimulation was applied to the buttocks (Amemiya et al, 2016(Amemiya et al, , 2013b(Amemiya et al, , 2013a, dorsal side of the body (Lind et al, 2016), feet (Kitazaki et al, 2019(Kitazaki et al, , 2016Kruijff et al, 2016;Matsuda et al, 2020;Nilsson et al, 2012;Nordahl et al, 2012;Väljamäe et al, 2009), the lower back and buttocks (Soave et al, 2020), torso (Rupert & Kolev, 2008) and waist (Tinga et al, 2018). Participants passively experienced the stimuli except in the experiments by Kruijff et al (2016) wherein participants controlled the movement of a virtual avatar by either leaning forward or by moving a joystick.…”
Section: Existing Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…In the 23 articles we found through our literature search, the application of tactile stimuli varied from applying forces to the body (Oishi et al, 2016), blowing wind (Churan et al, 2017;Feng et al, 2016;Seno, Ogawa, et al, 2011), stretching the skin (Horie et al, 2018) to vibro-tactile stimulation of various body parts. Vibro-tactile stimulation was applied to the buttocks (Amemiya et al, 2016(Amemiya et al, , 2013b(Amemiya et al, , 2013a, dorsal side of the body (Lind et al, 2016), feet (Kitazaki et al, 2019(Kitazaki et al, , 2016Kruijff et al, 2016;Matsuda et al, 2020;Nilsson et al, 2012;Nordahl et al, 2012;Väljamäe et al, 2009), the lower back and buttocks (Soave et al, 2020), torso (Rupert & Kolev, 2008) and waist (Tinga et al, 2018). Participants passively experienced the stimuli except in the experiments by Kruijff et al (2016) wherein participants controlled the movement of a virtual avatar by either leaning forward or by moving a joystick.…”
Section: Existing Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, some authors described the possibility of enhancing visually induced vection by multimodal stimulation (Kruijff et al, 2016;Riecke, Schulte-Pelkum, Caniard, et al, 2005) or that the perception of self-motion is a multisensory event (Oishi et al, 2016). Tinga et al (2018) described vection as "…the sensation of self-motion induced by moving sensory stimulation not corresponding to the veridical self motion." Väljamäe et al (2006) described vection as "…a sensation of actual movement relative to a stable surrounding environment", which predominantly aligns with the second definition of vection of Palmisano et al (2015).…”
Section: The Definition Of Vection and Participants' Task Instructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations