2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infant and adult perceptions of possible and impossible body movements: An eye-tracking study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
35
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
4
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The finding of a preference for impossible hand movements at birth is in line with the preference that older infants manifest for scrambled animated and static bodies (e.g., Christie & Slaughter, ; see review by Slaughter, Stone, & Reed, ) and impossible body movements (Morita et al., ; Reid et al., ). Infants' preference for impossible movements has been explained as being due to the fact that these movements are novel or surprising (Reid et al., ; Slaughter et al., ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The finding of a preference for impossible hand movements at birth is in line with the preference that older infants manifest for scrambled animated and static bodies (e.g., Christie & Slaughter, ; see review by Slaughter, Stone, & Reed, ) and impossible body movements (Morita et al., ; Reid et al., ). Infants' preference for impossible movements has been explained as being due to the fact that these movements are novel or surprising (Reid et al., ; Slaughter et al., ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
“…Eight-month-olds, in particular those with high fine motor skills, discriminated between biomechanically possible and impossible arm movements and preferred the impossible ones (Reid, Belsky, & Johnson, 2005). Similarly, 12-month-olds looked longer at the elbow of a virtual agent performing impossible, rather than possible, arm movements (Morita et al, 2012). Furthermore, it is not until the age of 18 months that infants distinguish between static pictures of typical and scrambled bodies (Slaughter & Heron, 2004;Slaughter, Heron, & Sim, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…A preference for the possible grip would be in line with earlier demonstrations of infants' preference for familiar motion patterns, such as human biological motion (Bertenthal, Proffitt, & Cutting, ; Bertenthal et al., ; Simion et al., ), biologically possible vs. impossible whole‐body movements (Christie & Slaughter, ), and movements that are already part of infants' motor repertoire (Sanefuji, Ohgami, & Hashiya, ). Conversely, a preference for the impossible grip would suggest that the movement is perceived as an unfamiliar, unexpected event as compared to the overly familiar possible movement, and would be in accord with earlier demonstrations of longer looking times to unfamiliar body shapes and movements (Christie & Slaughter, ; Geangu et al., ; Longhi et al., ; Morita et al., ; Reid et al., ; Slaughter et al., ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 76%
“…In typical development, newborn infants and toddlers have been shown to prefer biological motion over non-biological motion (Simion, Regolin, & Bulf, 2008) or inverted motion (Klin, Lin, Gorrindo, Ramsay, & Jones, 2009). The predisposition to attend to biological motion has been shown in typical development using multiple methods including, point-light display (Klin et al, 2009; Morita et al, 2012; Simion et al, 2008), schematic motion such as the Michotte “caterpillar” stimulus (Michotte, 1963; Schlottmann & Ray, 2010), or the motion of a single animated dot (Rutherford, Pennington, & Rogers, 2006; Schultz & Bulthoff, 2013). This early sensitivity and preference for animate motion has been difficult to reconcile with some research suggesting that individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have deficits in biological motion perception (Annaz et al, 2010; Annaz, Campbell, Coleman, Milne, & Swettenham, 2012; Blake, Turner, Smoski, Pozdol, & Stone, 2003; Centelles, Assaiante, Etchegoyhen, Bouvard, & Schmitz, 2013; Congiu, Schlottmann, & Ray, 2010; David et al, 2013; Herrington et al, 2007; Kaiser, Delmolino, Tanaka, & Shiffrar, 2010; Klin et al, 2009; Koldewyn, Whitney, & Rivera, 2010; Koldewyn, Whitney, & Rivera, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%