2014
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12247
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infants Segment Continuous Events Using Transitional Probabilities

Abstract: Throughout their first year, infants adeptly detect statistical structure in their environment. However, little is known about whether statistical learning is a primary mechanism for event segmentation. This study directly tests whether statistical learning alone is sufficient to segment continuous events. Twenty-eight 7- to 9-month-old infants viewed a sequence of continuous actions performed by a novel agent in which there were no transitional movements that could have constrained the possible upcoming actio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
64
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 96 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
3
64
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Children discriminate between the improbable and the impossible in fictional stories (Weisberg & Sobel, 2012; but see Shtulman, 2009; Shtulman & Carey, 2007), and even young infants can use probabilistic information to make inferences about whether outcomes are statistically likely or unlikely (Téglás & Bonatti, 2016; Téglás, Girotto, Gonzalez, & Bonatti, 2007; Téglás et al, 2011; Xu & Denison, 2009; Xu & Garcia, 2008). Infants can also track auditory and visual statistical patterns (e.g., Saffran et al, 1996; Stahl et al, 2014) and are sensitive to the degree to which those patterns are surprising (Kidd, Piantadosi, & Aslin, 2012, 2014). It is not yet known whether the degree of children’s surprise parametrically affects learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Children discriminate between the improbable and the impossible in fictional stories (Weisberg & Sobel, 2012; but see Shtulman, 2009; Shtulman & Carey, 2007), and even young infants can use probabilistic information to make inferences about whether outcomes are statistically likely or unlikely (Téglás & Bonatti, 2016; Téglás, Girotto, Gonzalez, & Bonatti, 2007; Téglás et al, 2011; Xu & Denison, 2009; Xu & Garcia, 2008). Infants can also track auditory and visual statistical patterns (e.g., Saffran et al, 1996; Stahl et al, 2014) and are sensitive to the degree to which those patterns are surprising (Kidd, Piantadosi, & Aslin, 2012, 2014). It is not yet known whether the degree of children’s surprise parametrically affects learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, in addition to violations of core knowledge (which are in place prior to the experimental session), infants detect violations to associative expectations acquired on-line over the course of a laboratory task. Infants not only look longer when these associative expectations are violated (e.g., Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996; Stahl, Romberg, Roseberry, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2014), but also exhibit differential brain responses to events they have recently established as surprising (Emberson, Richards, & Aslin, 2015; Kouider et al, 2015). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…And does this sensitivity to either single dimensions or feature chunks change across development? For example, both adults and infants track the statistics of human action sequences (Baldwin et al 2008, Monroy et al 2017, Stahl et al 2014). One can imagine that, in this situation, the details of each visual element would not only be less important than the gestalt of the action being performed, but would also be a distraction from the task at hand.…”
Section: Statistics Of What? the Primitives Over Which Statistics mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, younger infants, 6- and 8-month-olds, can detect a salient target action in a continuous action sequence (Hespos, Saylor, & Grossman, 2009). There is also evidence that 7- to 9-month-old infants can segment continuous events using transitional probabilities (Roseberry, Richie, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, & Shipley, 2011; Stahl, Romberg, Roseberry, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2014). Furthermore, studies show that salient action effects help parse continuous action into individual actions for 9- to 12-month-old infants (Elsner, Hauf, & Aschersleben, 2007; Verschoor, Paulus, Spape, Biro, & Hommel, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%