1992
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.2.385
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infants' sensitivity to effects of gravity on visible object motion.

Abstract: A preference method probed infants' perception of object motion on an inclined plane. Infants viewed videotaped events in which a ball rolled downward (or upward) while speeding up (or slowing down). Then infants were tested with events in which the ball moved in the opposite direction with appropriate or inappropriate acceleration. Infants aged 7 months, but not 5 months, looked longer at the test event with inappropriate acceleration, suggesting emerging sensitivity to gravity. A further study tested whether… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
68
4

Year Published

2005
2005
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
4
68
4
Order By: Relevance
“…But the motor system must store information about gravity: all land-dwelling creatures must plan movements that compensate for gravity. The developmental emergence of motor compensation for gravity (Von Hofsten and Spelke, 1985) occurs several months earlier than visual sensitivity to acceleration due to gravity (Kim and Spelke, 1992). At least, this order suggests that visual and motor information about gravity are acquired separately, and at most, this order supports the possibility that motor compensation for environmental forces may contribute to visual sensitivity to environmental forces.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…But the motor system must store information about gravity: all land-dwelling creatures must plan movements that compensate for gravity. The developmental emergence of motor compensation for gravity (Von Hofsten and Spelke, 1985) occurs several months earlier than visual sensitivity to acceleration due to gravity (Kim and Spelke, 1992). At least, this order suggests that visual and motor information about gravity are acquired separately, and at most, this order supports the possibility that motor compensation for environmental forces may contribute to visual sensitivity to environmental forces.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…In particular, target objects are frequently accelerated by Earth gravity (1g ϭ 9.81 m/s 2 ), as in free-fall or projectile motion. Visual information about gravitational acceleration seems to contribute to perception of causality and naturalness of inanimate motion (Kim and Spelke 1992;Twardy and Bingham 2002), perception of distance and size of falling objects (Watson et al 1992), perception of biological motion (Troje and Westhoff 2006;Vallortigara and Regolin 2006), perception of human body postures (Lopez et al 2009), judgments of time intervals (Grealy et al 2004;Huber and Krist 2004), and manual interception of falling targets (Lacquaniti and Maioli 1989a,b;McIntyre et al 2001;Zago et al 2004). However, the brain substrates for processing visual gravitational motion are still incompletely understood.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability to detect gravitational acceleration in visual motion can be demonstrated early in life. Between 5 and 7 months, infants begin to implicitly expect a downwardly moving object to accelerate and an upwardly moving object to decelerate (10). Therefore, there is ample evidence that gravitational acceleration is taken into account in visual perception and interceptive responses.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, visually guided interceptions of objects falling under gravity are accurately timed (6)(7)(8), in contrast with interceptions of objects dropped in microgravity (9). Furthermore, visual gravity cues contribute to perception of causality and naturalness of motion (10,11) and to perception of distance and size for falling objects (12) or biological motion (13). Gravity cues also influence the realism of special effects in cinematography (12).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%