1991
DOI: 10.2190/1uv0-uyql-7aqv-v6un
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Information-Seeking Triggered by Age

Abstract: Age labels may lead to the seeking of age-stereotypic information from target individuals. Using Carver and de la Garza's information-seeking procedure (which asked respondents to judge the relative importance of various kinds of information they would glean from drivers involved in an automobile accident for assigning accident responsibility), but extending it to a wider life span (16 to 91 years of age) and to both genders, we generalized earlier findings to a similar undergraduate sample (n = 437) in anothe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2007
2007

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This serves to illustrate the reverse side of the same coin-individuals are age conscious. Thus, age cues will trigger information seeking (Ng, Giles, & Moody, 1991), and individuals will search for age cues (Hummert, 1994). Being ageist (age conscious), individuals are susceptible to the influence of old-age stereotypes when communicating with older adults.…”
Section: Linguistic Routinization Of Discriminationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This serves to illustrate the reverse side of the same coin-individuals are age conscious. Thus, age cues will trigger information seeking (Ng, Giles, & Moody, 1991), and individuals will search for age cues (Hummert, 1994). Being ageist (age conscious), individuals are susceptible to the influence of old-age stereotypes when communicating with older adults.…”
Section: Linguistic Routinization Of Discriminationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This effect has been noted particularly with older adults, whose symptoms are treated as a "normal" part of old age, as opposed to being treated in a way that would be routine for younger patients (Hamel et al, 2000;Rayburn & Stonecypher, 1996;Uncapher & Arean, 2000). The way in which information seeking can be driven by stereotypes has been documented (Ng, Giles, & Moody, 1991). Hence, further attention to the role of stereotypes in doctor-patient encounters is warranted (Greene & Adelman, 2001).…”
Section: Primary Identitiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies provide evidence for the claim that age stereotyping, perceptions, and expectations about age-associated communication behavior is not a one-way street with elders as the sole recipients of stereotypical reactions and age-adjusted talk (Williams & Giles, 1998). A study by Ng, Moody, and Giles (1991) asked respondents to formulate questions to put to targets of varying ages (i.e., aged 16 to 91) who had ostensibly had a car accident, the cause of which was ambiguous (see also Franklyn-Stokes, Harriman, Giles, & Coupland, 1988). Respondents rated questions about health, physical condition, and mental alertness as most appropriate for the older protagonists whereas for younger protagonists, questions about alcohol consumption and speeding were among those deemed most appropriate.…”
Section: Stereotypes and Ageism Toward Youthmentioning
confidence: 99%