2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.005
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Integrating multisensory information across external and motor-based frames of reference

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In our study, significant differences were found between the MT and VT groups and between the MT-R and VT groups ( p < .001 for both) (Figure 2). A previous study demonstrated that the higher the sense of body ownership, the greater the change in perceived hand position before and after the introduction of MT, and the two tended to be the same (Liu & Medina, 2018). In this experiment, the VAS (converted to a continuous scale from 0 to 100) for the group showed significant perceptual changes after MT ( M = 87.3 , SD = 11.7).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In our study, significant differences were found between the MT and VT groups and between the MT-R and VT groups ( p < .001 for both) (Figure 2). A previous study demonstrated that the higher the sense of body ownership, the greater the change in perceived hand position before and after the introduction of MT, and the two tended to be the same (Liu & Medina, 2018). In this experiment, the VAS (converted to a continuous scale from 0 to 100) for the group showed significant perceptual changes after MT ( M = 87.3 , SD = 11.7).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Each condition is named as P x V y , with P representing the proprioceptive position and V the visual position of the hand, and x and y denoting the distance of proprioceptive and visual position from the mirror respectively. Previous studies have shown that a mismatch of 6" between the proprioceptive and visual hand position in the mirror box reliably induced shifts in felt hand position toward the visual position (Liu & Medina, 2018;Medina et al, 2015). In each condition, participants were touched on the dorsum of the hidden left hand and reported where on the skin surface they felt the stimulus (see Procedures below).…”
Section: Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The mirror box was created in an attempt to treat patients with intractable phantom limb pain, particularly patients who felt their lost limb was clenched, with the fingernails digging into the skin (Ramachandran & Rogers-Ramachandran, 1996); watching the contralateral spared limb reflected in the mirror created the illusion of watching the lost limb and therefore, to watch it unclench, which was meant to alleviate the pain. The mirror box is often used to explore multisensory integration by manipulating visual, tactile, and proprioceptive information about the hands (e.g., Cardini & Longo, 2016;Holmes et al, 2004;Liu & Medina, 2018;Sadibolova & Longo, 2014;Romano et al, 2013). A typical Six finger illusion 5 mirror box setup is shown in Figure 1.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%