1969
DOI: 10.1037/h0028119
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
58
0

Year Published

1972
1972
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 101 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
3
58
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The notions were confirmed in experiments on ''nonaging foreperiod durations'' where the conditional probability of target onset, given the passage of time, remained constant by manipulating the proportion of trials in the various foreperiod durations, as is explained shortly. The results of these experiments indicate that when foreperiod durations are nonaging, the usual trend for RT reduction with increasing foreperiod duration has been eliminated or even slightly reversed (Baumeister & Jubert, 1969;Naatanen, 1970;Naatanen & Merisalo, 1977;Nickerson & Burnham, 1969).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The notions were confirmed in experiments on ''nonaging foreperiod durations'' where the conditional probability of target onset, given the passage of time, remained constant by manipulating the proportion of trials in the various foreperiod durations, as is explained shortly. The results of these experiments indicate that when foreperiod durations are nonaging, the usual trend for RT reduction with increasing foreperiod duration has been eliminated or even slightly reversed (Baumeister & Jubert, 1969;Naatanen, 1970;Naatanen & Merisalo, 1977;Nickerson & Burnham, 1969).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Specifically, RT was predicted to become shorter by increasing the CTI only when the foreperiod durations are aging, that is, when the CTIs are either mostly long or equally probable. Such a trend was not predicted for the group for whom most CTIs were short, and the foreperiod durations were nonaging (Baumeister & Jubert, 1969;Naatanen, 1970;Naatanen & Merisalo, 1977;Nickerson & Burnham, 1969).…”
Section: Possible Interaction Between the Prediction Of Target Onset mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…This was done by simply switching the proportions of trials with the first and last foreperiods, creating a foreperiod distribution called accelerated aging (Baumeister & Joubert, 1969). The total number of trials remained the same as in Experiment 1, but now instead of having equal, 50 % chances for the target to appear in each of the foreperiods, the probability for the target to appear in the first, 100-ms foreperiod was 12.5 % (64 out of the total 512 trials; see also the calculation of probabilities in the "Accelerated Aging" column of Table 1); the probability for target appearance in the second, 500-ms foreperiod was 28.6 % (128 out of the remaining 448 possible trials); and the probability for appearance in the final, 900-ms foreperiod was 80 % (256 trials out of the remaining 320 trials).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This constant probability of target appearance throughout the different SOAs makes the cue noninformative as to when the target will appear. In addition to the nonaging condition, we used an accelerated-aging condition, which was the reverse of the nonaging condition (Baumeister & Joubert, 1969). This condition is characterized by an accelerated probability of target appearance as SOA increases.…”
Section: Temporal Information Provided By An Exogenous Cuementioning
confidence: 99%