Handbook of Neurobehavioral Genetics and Phenotyping 2017
DOI: 10.1002/9781118540770.ch8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interval‐timing Protocols and Their Relevancy to the Study of Temporal Cognition and Neurobehavioral Genetics

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 336 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This more rapid depletion in dopamine function is accompanied by our sense that the external world is going faster, when in fact it may be our internal clock that is going slower, thereby suggesting to us that sequences of events are occurring in a shorter amount of time than would typically be expected (see Cooper and Erickson, 2002 ). An example of this “fatigue effect” was reported by Malapani et al ( 1998 ) in their study of Parkinson’s disease patients and aged-matched controls trained and then tested on a duration reproduction procedure without feedback (see Yin et al, 2016a for procedural details). Over the course of a 2 h session, healthy aged participants showed proportional rightward shifts in the reproduction of 8-s and 21-s target durations that increased as a function of 30-min session blocks as illustrated in Figure 1 .…”
Section: Age and Timing Performance: Decline Preservation And Compementioning
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This more rapid depletion in dopamine function is accompanied by our sense that the external world is going faster, when in fact it may be our internal clock that is going slower, thereby suggesting to us that sequences of events are occurring in a shorter amount of time than would typically be expected (see Cooper and Erickson, 2002 ). An example of this “fatigue effect” was reported by Malapani et al ( 1998 ) in their study of Parkinson’s disease patients and aged-matched controls trained and then tested on a duration reproduction procedure without feedback (see Yin et al, 2016a for procedural details). Over the course of a 2 h session, healthy aged participants showed proportional rightward shifts in the reproduction of 8-s and 21-s target durations that increased as a function of 30-min session blocks as illustrated in Figure 1 .…”
Section: Age and Timing Performance: Decline Preservation And Compementioning
confidence: 90%
“…Data are taken from test sessions described by Malapani et al ( 1998 ) and Rakitin et al ( 1998 ). See Lustig and Meck ( 2005 ), Lake and Meck ( 2013 ), and Yin et al ( 2016a ) for a review of the benefits of the peak-interval procedure.…”
Section: Age and Timing Performance: Decline Preservation And Compementioning
confidence: 99%
“…MSNs are trained over successive trials by synaptic plasticity mechanisms to function as detectors of unique patterns of input that are related to specific target durations paired with reinforcement (Dallérac et al, 2017). A major strength of the model is that it accounts for the scalar property which is the hallmark of interval timing (Allman et al, 2014;Yin et al, 2017). The first component of the scalar property requires that the mean measures of the timed behavior vary linearly, and usually accurately, with imposed temporal standards (i.e., target durations).…”
Section: Computational Properties Of the Striatummentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Well, one common way of achieving this is to omit the reward sporadically so as to capture the full temporal profile of the anticipatory responses (called peak trials), which is exactly what Toda et al [3] did: they observed that the average rate of licking exhibited a bell-shaped curve as a function of trial time, with its peak around the actual target time. As in other domains [8], however, averaging the data causes artifacts: the pattern of responding in individual trials is not a bell-curve but a boxcar demarcated by the abrupt increases and decrements in response rate [9]. Toda et al [3] carried out a not only more appropriate but also theory-driven level of analysis, and quantified the delays at which mice started and stopped anticipating the reward delivery in individual peak trials ( Figure 1B).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Probably the most famous examples are from our own order, the primates, whose exploitative talents feed our curiosity about technological evolution in humans. Evidence exists in the animal kingdom for tool kits and sets [9][10][11], material preferences [12] and design modifications [10,13]. Yet…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%