2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intuitive proportion judgment in number-line estimation: Converging evidence from multiple tasks

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
38
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
2
38
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Considerable evidence against this view has emerged in recent years. Researchers using a variety of methods have shown that performance in these estimation tasks is shaped by multiple cognitive processes, strategies, and/or computations that must prevent direct access to the forms of our numerical magnitude representations (e.g., Barth & Paladino, ; Barth et al., ; Chesney & Mathews, ; Cohen & Blanc‐Goldhammer, ; Cohen & Quinlan, ; Cohen & Sarnecka, ; Friso‐van den Bos et al., ; Hurst, Monahan, Heller, & Cordes, ; Link, Huber, Nuerk, & Moeller, ; Peeters, Degrande, Ebersbach, Verschaffel, & Luwel, ; Reinert, Huber, Nuerk, & Moeller, ; Rips, ; Rouder & Geary, ; Slusser, Santiago, & Barth, ; Slusser & Barth, ; Sullivan & Barner, ; Sullivan, Juhasz, Slattery, & Barth, ; see also Cantlon, Cordes, Libertus, & Brannon, ). But despite disagreement about what cognitive and developmental theories should be built upon number line estimates, researchers in both groups described above have generally assumed that the overall numerical magnitudes of the presented targets do ultimately drive task performance (cf.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Considerable evidence against this view has emerged in recent years. Researchers using a variety of methods have shown that performance in these estimation tasks is shaped by multiple cognitive processes, strategies, and/or computations that must prevent direct access to the forms of our numerical magnitude representations (e.g., Barth & Paladino, ; Barth et al., ; Chesney & Mathews, ; Cohen & Blanc‐Goldhammer, ; Cohen & Quinlan, ; Cohen & Sarnecka, ; Friso‐van den Bos et al., ; Hurst, Monahan, Heller, & Cordes, ; Link, Huber, Nuerk, & Moeller, ; Peeters, Degrande, Ebersbach, Verschaffel, & Luwel, ; Reinert, Huber, Nuerk, & Moeller, ; Rips, ; Rouder & Geary, ; Slusser, Santiago, & Barth, ; Slusser & Barth, ; Sullivan & Barner, ; Sullivan, Juhasz, Slattery, & Barth, ; see also Cantlon, Cordes, Libertus, & Brannon, ). But despite disagreement about what cognitive and developmental theories should be built upon number line estimates, researchers in both groups described above have generally assumed that the overall numerical magnitudes of the presented targets do ultimately drive task performance (cf.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent empirical evidence suggests that number line estimation tasks entail judgments of proportions (Barth & Paladino, 2011;Slusser & Barth, 2017).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Estimation strategies tell us how students approach the number line task; students' accuracy on the number line task reveals whether those estimation strategies are successful. For example, when they are asked to place whole numbers on number lines, older students and adults rely on both implicit (e.g., the midpoint) and explicit reference points (e.g., labelled endpoints) to guide their number line estimates (Ashcraft & Moore, 2012;Barth & Paladino, 2011;Di Lonardo et al, 2019;Slusser & Barth, 2017). Notably, when students rely on implicit reference points they also use proportional reasoning skills (e.g., 45 is a little smaller than 50 so it should be placed just to the left of the midpoint on a 0-100 number line) (Ashcraft & Moore, 2012;Barth & Paladino, 2011;Newman & Berger, 1984;Peeters et al, 2016;Schneider et al, 2018).…”
Section: Number Line Estimation Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%