2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-6261.2011.01701.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

IQ and Stock Market Participation

Abstract: An individual's IQ stanine, measured early in adult life, is monotonically related to his stock market participation decision later in life. The high correlation between IQ and participation, which exists even among the 10% most affluent individuals, controls for wealth, income, and other demographic and occupational information. Supplemental data from siblings are used with both an instrumental variables approach and paired difference regressions to show that our results apply to both females and males, and t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

32
288
0
3

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 596 publications
(338 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
32
288
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Viewed in this light, the fact that our measurement-error-adjusted estimates suggest a one-standard-deviation increase in cognitive ability is associated with up to a 0.3-standard-deviation increase in risk attitudes is remarkable. The association between IQ and risk attitudes is also consistent with the recent findings of Grinblatt et al (2011), who found that stock market participation increases with higher IQ. Further, the association is very significant for all risk variables, including Risk General and Risk Financial (which involve simple self-rated scales), thus suggesting the association is not only or primarily driven by the fact that measurement noise may be higher for individuals with lower cognitive ability (Andersson et al 2013).…”
Section: Model Without Measurement Errorsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Viewed in this light, the fact that our measurement-error-adjusted estimates suggest a one-standard-deviation increase in cognitive ability is associated with up to a 0.3-standard-deviation increase in risk attitudes is remarkable. The association between IQ and risk attitudes is also consistent with the recent findings of Grinblatt et al (2011), who found that stock market participation increases with higher IQ. Further, the association is very significant for all risk variables, including Risk General and Risk Financial (which involve simple self-rated scales), thus suggesting the association is not only or primarily driven by the fact that measurement noise may be higher for individuals with lower cognitive ability (Andersson et al 2013).…”
Section: Model Without Measurement Errorsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…This figure is within the estimate by Coval, Hirshleifer, and Shumway (2005), who find persistent superior performance among the investors in the top decile. This estimate is also within the bounds of Grinblatt, Keloharju, and Linnainmaa (2010), who find superior performance among the 4% of individuals who receive the highest score on an IQ test. These estimates must, however, be compared with some caution.…”
Section: Downloaded Frommentioning
confidence: 53%
“…Performance heterogeneity. Studies by Barber et al (2005), Coval, Hirshleifer, and Shumway (2005), Bauer, Cosemans, and Eichholtz (2007), Goetzmann and Kumar (2008), Ivkovi膰, Sialm, and Weisbenner (2008), Nicolosi, Peng, and Zhu (2009), Grinblatt, Keloharju, and Linnainmaa (2010), and Seru, Shumway, and Stoffman (2010) find that a small number of individual investors outperform the market or their peers. Harris and Schultz (1998) show that some individual investors who use Nasdaq's Small Order Execution System for day trading are better than others.…”
Section: Stylized Facts About Household Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…4.1), several contributions lacking an observable measure of financial capabilities exploit this evidence and use respondents' demographics in order to capture their financial literacy. Corresponding proxies for financial sophistication used in the literature include (disposable) income and wealth (Dhar and Zhu 2006;Vissing-Jorgensen 2003;Calvet et al 2007Calvet et al , 2009) as well as age (Calvet et al 2007(Calvet et al , 2009Georgarakos and Pasini 2011), educational attainment (Christiansen et al 2008;Calvet et al 2007Calvet et al , 2009), professional status (Calvet et al 2009), and even IQ (Grinblatt et al 2011(Grinblatt et al , 2012. Likewise, both Chalmers and Reuter (2012) and Hackethal et al (2012) use subsets of these demographics to proxy for financial literacy in their analyses.…”
Section: Socio-demographic Proxiesmentioning
confidence: 99%