2003
DOI: 10.3758/bf03196544
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review

Abstract: In a recent meta-analysis, Lucas (2000) concluded that there is strong evidence of an overall pure semantic priming effect but no evidence of priming based purely on association. In the present review, I critically examine the individual studies claiming evidence of featural and associative relations in semantic memory. The most important conclusion is that automatic priming appears to be due to both association strength and feature overlap. Mediated associates provide the strongest evidence of automatic assoc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

27
455
1
6

Year Published

2008
2008
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 403 publications
(489 citation statements)
references
References 139 publications
(281 reference statements)
27
455
1
6
Order By: Relevance
“…We thus conclude that categorical priming effects of affective and semantic congruency are not mediated by differences in target processing (spreading activation) but instead reflect effects of response facilitation and conflict (cf. also Hutchison, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We thus conclude that categorical priming effects of affective and semantic congruency are not mediated by differences in target processing (spreading activation) but instead reflect effects of response facilitation and conflict (cf. also Hutchison, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most phrasal associates (e.g., SPIDER WEB; Hodgson, 1991;Hutchison, 2003;Williams, 1996) are understood via relational integration, and moreover, familiar noun compounds are understood more quickly than novel compounds (Wisniewski & Murphy, 2005).…”
Section: Integrative Priming Integrative Primingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Associative priming generally increases from brief to long SOAs (see Hutchison, 2003, Table 3; see also Hutchison et al, 2008 Coolen and colleagues (1991) in their study of relational integration, and this late SOA is known to produce associative priming (den Heyer et al, 1985;Neely, 1991;Perea & Rosa, 2002). The experiment therefore had a 3 (Prime: baseline, associative, integrative; within-participants) × 2 (SOA: 500, 2000 ms; between-participants) mixed design using a standard LDT.…”
Section: Integrative Priming 13mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such associative networks (see Figure 1) are common in modeling human thought (e.g., Anderson, 1976Anderson, , 1983Reder, Park, & Kieffaber, 2009) and involve a variety of semantic relationships (e.g., temporal continguity, spatial proximity, feature overlap, shared perceptual properties, category membership, antonymity, synonymy) (e.g., Wu & Barsalou, 2009). Although it is often difficult to discriminate associative strength and semantic overlap (see Hutchison, 2003), the use of associatively related lists (e.g., as in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott [DRM] paradigm; Deese, 1959;Roediger & McDermott, 1995) has become the sine qua non for researchers interested in the study of memory accuracy (i.e., the relationship between rates of correct recollection and error rates). Indeed, the use of the DRM paradigm to study memory accuracy is appropriate because these lists contain many of the semantic relations found in human thought, not just taxonomic (categorical) relations (see Brainerd, Yang, Reyna, Howe, & Mills, 2008b;Howe et al, 2009bHowe et al, , 2009cPark et al, 2005).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%