The automation of vehicles can be divided into several steps (SAE International, 2018). Level 0 contains no automation at all. Level 1 contains driving assistance that is either longitudinal or lateral. For instance, cruise control or adaptive cruise control would assist longitudinal vehicle control by holding or adapting velocity. At partial automation (Level 2) both lateral and longitudinal driving assistance systems are combined to support the driver. An example is the combination of adaptive cruise control and lane centering functions. In combination these two assistance functions support the driver in staying in the lane and maintaining a certain speed. However, at this level of automation, the driver remains the labels entail (Flach, 1995;Woods, 1993) and shifting scientific endeavors away from observable human performance (Dekker & Hollnagel, 2004).
The Loop Defined From Perception-Action Theory PerspectiveGenerally speaking, perception-action theory originates from James Gibson's seminal work on understanding drivers' and pilots' capacity to control movement adequately (Gibson et al., 1955;Gibson & Crooks, 1938). Building on Gibson's later work (Gibson, 1986) the perception-action theory approach investigates how drivers locomote by interacting with the environment through the process of perception and action loops (Bootsma, 1998;Harrison et al., 2016;Mathieu et al., 2017a). For example, the optical expansion of a stop sign in the retina of drivers guides their braking actions towards it (DeLucia et al., 2016;Lee, 1976;Morando et al., 2016). In turn, the action of braking changes the optical expansion of the stop sign relative to the driver, thereby closing the perception-action loop. Iterations of these perception-action loops allow driving behavior to emerge (Harrison et al., 2016;Mathieu et al., 2017a).In order to understand how drivers can be kept in the loop, it is necessary to understand two key processes underlying the perception-action loop: perceptual attunement and perceptual-motor calibration. Perceptual attunement entails that a driver is exposed to -and able to detect and track -the specifying (i.e. task-relevant) visual information (Bootsma, 1998). In the vehicle braking example above, drivers are perceptually attuned to visual variables such as looming, specifying the relative distance to other vehicles (Fajen & Devaney, 2006). In lane keeping tasks, drivers have been shown to perceptually attune to visual variables such as optical flow, bearing-and slay angles (Li & Chen, 2010), or the inner point of approaching curves (Land & Lee, 1994).To close the perception-action loop, drivers must be proficient in scaling their motor responses adequately to this information, a process termed perceptualmotor calibration (Brand & de Oliveira, 2017;Van Andel et al., 2017). Perceptualmotor calibration is not static but dynamically dependent on the situation. Drivers have been shown to need a period of perceptual-motor calibration when tailwind is introduced in a vehicle braking task (Fajen, 2005b) or wh...