1992
DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.504
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Knowledge of connectors as cohesion devices in text: A comparative study of native-English and English-as-a-second-language speakers.

Abstract: Readers' understanding of the functions of 4 types of logical connectors (additives, causals, adversatives, and sequentials) was examined with a multiple-choice, rational cloze procedure. Native English-language speakers and speakers of English as a second language read informational passages from which specific connectors had been removed. They selected a connector and gave verbal justifications (Experiments 1 and 3) and confidence ratings (Experiments 2 and 3). Patterns of correct responses were similar acro… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
59
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
2
59
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There are studies which explore whether participants produce well integrated texts using discourse markers (Cain, 2003;Cain, & Oakhill, 1996) or choose the best discourse marker between some options (Cain, Patson, & Andrews, 2005;Goldman, & Murray, 1992). In these cases participants have to be writers or analysts.…”
Section: Rhetorical Competence Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…There are studies which explore whether participants produce well integrated texts using discourse markers (Cain, 2003;Cain, & Oakhill, 1996) or choose the best discourse marker between some options (Cain, Patson, & Andrews, 2005;Goldman, & Murray, 1992). In these cases participants have to be writers or analysts.…”
Section: Rhetorical Competence Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Knowledge of logical connectives was also found to facilitate comprehension of the texts from unfamiliar domains. This is because, without the sufficient content schemata, readers tend to resort to the surface aspects of the texts and linguistic devices to help them to make inferences about the relationships between propositions in the text (Goldman & Murray, 1992). Experimental data also indicate that explicit marking of coherence relations leads to faster text processing (Haberlandt, 1982;Millis & Just, 1994;Sanders & Norman, 2000) and better recall of sentence-pairs (Caron, Micko & Thuring, 1988).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several conditions, however, must be fulfilled for this effect to take place. According to Goldman and Murray (1992), in order to take advantage of the linguistic markers in the text, readers must lack the requisite linguistic knowledge and content schema to infer implicit coherence relations. High-skilled readers with sufficient background knowledge, good understanding of the text structure, and a high level of language proficiency are able to construct coherent mental representations of the texts, even when logical relationships are not explicitly signaled.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…the causal fa introduces the second clause that provides the cause or the reason of the event stated in the first clause of the compound element. Consider the following example = ‫ضؼف‬ ‫انبكاء‬ ‫فاٌ‬ ‫تبك‬ ‫ال‬ ( 13 ) laa tabki fa?inna al-bukaa?a ḍa9f-un.=Don't cry because crying is weakness.…”
Section: Responses To the Consequential 'θUmma'mentioning
confidence: 99%