2006
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.854
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lack of set size effects in spatial updating: Evidence for offline updating.

Abstract: Four experiments required participants to keep track of the locations of (i.e., update) 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, 10, or 15 target objects after rotating. Across all conditions, updating was unaffected by set size. Although some traditional set size effects (i.e., a linear increase of latency with memory load) were observed under some conditions, these effects were independent of the updating process. Patterns of data and participant strategies were inconsistent with the common view of spatial updating as an online pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
34
2

Year Published

2007
2007
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
3
34
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The evidence on the capacity limits of spatial updating is mixed. Results of at least one study indicate that spatial updating deteriorates in accuracy as the number of objects increases (Wang et al, 2006), whereas findings from other studies indicate that spatial updating is capacity-free (Rieser and Rider, 1991;Hodgson and Waller, 2006). This pattern of results has led some researchers to distinguish two forms of updating, one that occurs on-line and relies on working memory and another that occurs off-line and relies on long-term memory (e.g., Amorim et al, 1997;Cornell and Greidanus, 2006;Hodgson and Waller, 2006).…”
Section: Navigation and Spatial Updatingmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…The evidence on the capacity limits of spatial updating is mixed. Results of at least one study indicate that spatial updating deteriorates in accuracy as the number of objects increases (Wang et al, 2006), whereas findings from other studies indicate that spatial updating is capacity-free (Rieser and Rider, 1991;Hodgson and Waller, 2006). This pattern of results has led some researchers to distinguish two forms of updating, one that occurs on-line and relies on working memory and another that occurs off-line and relies on long-term memory (e.g., Amorim et al, 1997;Cornell and Greidanus, 2006;Hodgson and Waller, 2006).…”
Section: Navigation and Spatial Updatingmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…In fact, performance is typically comparable from these two standpoints even when the movement towards the novel standpoint is done without vision (but see Hodgson and Waller 2006). Participants are also quite good at continually pointing to a target during physical movement (Loomis et al 1992).…”
Section: Updating Of Egocentric Relationsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Then s/he must compute a response vector using that reference frame. Perhaps this computation takes place using an offline updating process (Amorim et al 1997;Hodgson and Waller 2006). One form of off-line updating that could be used is that of mentally simulating movement in space and deliberating observing its consequence on imagined egocentric locations.…”
Section: Sensorimotor Interference When Reasoning About Immediate Envmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Then as task demands direct attention to particular points within the bound representation, the egocentric vector conception of the representation might give way to a richer conception in which objects are encoded relative to each other. This richer representation may reside offline during updating as suggested by Amorim, Glasauer, Corpinot, and Berthoz (1997) and Hodgson and Waller (2006), and it may be supported partly or wholly by egocentric imagery, but it undoubtedly contains a wealth of spatial information about the relative location of its component parts, independent of the location of one's body. It is in this sense that the bound representations may be considered allocentric.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%