2001
DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00120-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
230
4
3

Year Published

2006
2006
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 278 publications
(247 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
10
230
4
3
Order By: Relevance
“…At face value, the development of sensorimotor and verbal spatial abilities seems to be a case of discontinuous developmentsen sorimotor spatial abilities emerge in infancy and show rapid change thereafter (e.g., Acredolo, 1985;Newcombe, Huttenlocher, & Learmonth, 1999;Piaget, 1954), whereas verbal spatial abilities emerge much later, reaching profi ciency by 5 to 7 years of age (e.g., Craton, Elicker, Plumert, & Pick, 1990;Hermer-Vazquez, Spelke, & Katsnelson, 1999;Plumert, Ewert, & Spear, 1995;Plumert & Nichols-Whitehead, 1996). Consistent with this discontinuous view, Hermer-Vazquez, Moffet, and Munkholm (2001) showed that 5-to 7-year-old children encode spatial relations in new ways once they become profi cient at using spatial language. In particular, spatial language helps children combine geometric and nongeometric, featural information following a disorientation procedure that disrupts children's ability to use dead-reckoning to fi nd a hidden object.…”
Section: Developmental Changes In Spatial Memorymentioning
confidence: 92%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…At face value, the development of sensorimotor and verbal spatial abilities seems to be a case of discontinuous developmentsen sorimotor spatial abilities emerge in infancy and show rapid change thereafter (e.g., Acredolo, 1985;Newcombe, Huttenlocher, & Learmonth, 1999;Piaget, 1954), whereas verbal spatial abilities emerge much later, reaching profi ciency by 5 to 7 years of age (e.g., Craton, Elicker, Plumert, & Pick, 1990;Hermer-Vazquez, Spelke, & Katsnelson, 1999;Plumert, Ewert, & Spear, 1995;Plumert & Nichols-Whitehead, 1996). Consistent with this discontinuous view, Hermer-Vazquez, Moffet, and Munkholm (2001) showed that 5-to 7-year-old children encode spatial relations in new ways once they become profi cient at using spatial language. In particular, spatial language helps children combine geometric and nongeometric, featural information following a disorientation procedure that disrupts children's ability to use dead-reckoning to fi nd a hidden object.…”
Section: Developmental Changes In Spatial Memorymentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Indeed, on the basis of previous studies showing that verbal responses remain stable during short-term delays (e.g., Bridgeman et al, 1997;Brungart et al, 2000) and that 7-year-olds are proficient at spatial language (Hermer-Vazquez et al, 2001), we expected participants to be relatively accurate in this task. It is also possible, however, that participants will show geometric bi ases on the choice task.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Here we want to add an additional core cognitive ability: the ability to represent ordered relations. In addition, and based on previous arguments ( [10,11],e.g., [12,13]), it is hypothesized here that acquired symbolic representations, i.e., language, provides a medium, in which information from these separated core domainspecific systems (i.e., quantity and ordinality) can be combined. Accordingly, new representations that depend on language or acquired symbols are expected to include concepts related to old core or innate components (such as quantity and ordinal information), but which involve also new linguistic combinations (such as the direction of writing).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Humans, like other species, readily use geometric information to guide their navigation after they are disoriented (2, 6), but their proficient and flexible landmark use does not emerge until age 5 under many circumstances, and its emergence correlates with the productive mastery of the phrases "left of X" and "right of X" (2,(17)(18)(19). Further, adults' landmark use after disorientation is impaired when they engage in a language repetition task in a small, enclosed environment (1,20).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%