2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.01.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning in settings with partial feedback and the wavy recency effect of rare events

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
34
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
3
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…(Nevo & Erev, ) In decisions from experience, agents are commonly engaged in choice inertia (repeating the most recently chosen option (Ashby & Teodorescu, ; Biele et al, ): Model‐based analysis of behavior in this paradigm estimated the likelihood of inertia at over 70% of the choices. (Plonsky & Erev, ) According to “surprise triggers change,” rare (thus, surprising) events increase the likelihood that choice inertia discontinues. Therefore, rare events tend to increase the likelihood for a switch in choice (beyond the pure effect of the reinforcement).…”
Section: Why Is the Effect Of Forgone‐worse Information Different In mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…(Nevo & Erev, ) In decisions from experience, agents are commonly engaged in choice inertia (repeating the most recently chosen option (Ashby & Teodorescu, ; Biele et al, ): Model‐based analysis of behavior in this paradigm estimated the likelihood of inertia at over 70% of the choices. (Plonsky & Erev, ) According to “surprise triggers change,” rare (thus, surprising) events increase the likelihood that choice inertia discontinues. Therefore, rare events tend to increase the likelihood for a switch in choice (beyond the pure effect of the reinforcement).…”
Section: Why Is the Effect Of Forgone‐worse Information Different In mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, consider participants who choose the status quo option in Problem 4RD and get feedback concerning outcomes worse than their obtained outcomes (i.e., Conditions All‐Forgone and Only‐Worse‐Forgone) such that if a −10 is happened to be drawn in the risky option, they would observe it as a forgone loss. According to surprise triggers change, this surprising event would then increase the likelihood they would switch their choice to the risky option in the next trial (i.e., a negative recency effect (Plonsky et al, ; Plonsky & Erev, )). That is, unlike in other problems, in rare disaster environments, exposure to worse forgone outcomes would tend to increase the risky rates.…”
Section: Why Is the Effect Of Forgone‐worse Information Different In mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The majority of narratives occurred during participants' postsecondary studies. In part, the sharing of these particular narratives may reflect participants' increased knowledge and abilities to recognize mental health challenges, chronological proximity to the onset of mental health challenges, relational proximity to relevant details and events, and recency effects (i.e., high memory for recent events; Plonsky & Erev, 2017).…”
Section: Content Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The outcome of 32 may be so tempting that the participant stops sampling and chooses Option A for the final decision. Likewise, in the negative domain (e.g., Problem 2: A −3 B 0 A −3 B 0 A −3 B 0 A −3 B −32 ), a participant might stop sampling after a large negative outcome, akin to the “hot‐stove” effect (Denrell, ; Plonsky & Erev, ).…”
Section: The Relation Between Optional Stopping and Final Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…, a participant might stop sampling after a large negative outcome, akin to the "hot-stove" effect (Denrell, 2007;Plonsky & Erev, 2017).…”
Section: The Relation Between Optional Stopping and Final Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%