2010
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.746
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning to control ethnic intergroup bias in childhood

Abstract: Abstract

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

6
62
0
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
6
62
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Additionally, we build on existing research that shows the relevance of intergroup processes for children and adolescents' social interactions (see Fitzroy & Rutland, 2010;Killen & Rutland, 2011;Nesdale, 2008;Sierksma et al, 2014), and extend it further by applying this approach to bystander intentions during incidents of intergroup verbal aggression.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Additionally, we build on existing research that shows the relevance of intergroup processes for children and adolescents' social interactions (see Fitzroy & Rutland, 2010;Killen & Rutland, 2011;Nesdale, 2008;Sierksma et al, 2014), and extend it further by applying this approach to bystander intentions during incidents of intergroup verbal aggression.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, crossethnic friendships may also foster positive development through decreasing perceived discrimination and intergroup anxiety and increasing social competence. Similarly, future research may incorporate the role of perspective-taking and theory of mind in the association between cross-ethnic friendships and well-being, since such social-cognitive abilities may moderate negative outgroup attitudes (e.g., Fitzroy & Rutland, 2010) and therefore promote well-being.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, a mutual sharing of information among different ethnic group members is likely to promote social-cognitive skills such as perspective-taking and empathy and thereby facilitate children's social interactions in multi-ethnic settings (Turner et al, 2007). In fact, cross-ethnic friendships improve perspective taking (Eisenberg et al, 2009), resilience (Bagci, Rutland, Kumsahiro, Smith, & Blumberg, 2014), and social skills (Lease & Blake, 2005) and therefore equip children with necessary skills to moderate prejudicial attitudes (Fitzroy & Rutland, 2010). Second, this kind of information exchange across ethnic boundaries also provides children with a varied range of reference points to evaluate themselves (Antonio, 2004), and functions like 'social capital', providing children with opportunities to access important information about being successful in an ethnically diverse society (Crosnoe, Cavanagh, & Elder, 2003).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because these studies did not measure children's awareness of or desire to comply with social norms, it is not possible to say that young children never demonstrate susceptibility to social desirability. In fact, children as young as six may be aware of social desirability norms and respond in socially desirable ways under some circumstances ( e.g ., when accountable to teachers and classmates; FitzRoy & Rutland, 2010; Rutland, Cameron, Milne, & McGeorge, 2005). Additionally, even preschool-aged children conceal opinions that may be perceived as socially undesirable, such as negative evaluations of someone's drawing or a gift that one has received (Fu & Lee, 2007; Talwar, Murphy, & Lee, 2007).…”
Section: How Does the Dissociated State Of Attitudes Develop?mentioning
confidence: 99%