2010
DOI: 10.1037/a0020377
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning to read and spell in Persian: A cross-sectional study from Grades 1 to 4.

Abstract: We investigated the reading and spelling development of 140 Persian children attending Grades 1-4 in Iran. Persian has very consistent letter-sound correspondences, but it varies in transparency because 3 of its 6 vowel phonemes are not marked with letters. Persian also varies in spelling consistency because 6 phonemes have more than one orthographic representation. We tested whether lexicality effects-an advantage of words over nonwords-would be affected be reading transparency and spelling consistency. We fo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…sequence. The sequential order of the root’s consonants, as they appear in their derivatives, allows for a well-defined orthographic representation (Ehri, 1992, 2005, 2017; Perfetti, 1992; Rahbari & Sénéchal, 2010; Share, 1999; Taha & Saiegh-Haddad, 2017; Tibi, 2016). As a further illustration, Table 1 shows examples of words derived from three different root structures.…”
Section: Arabic Morphologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…sequence. The sequential order of the root’s consonants, as they appear in their derivatives, allows for a well-defined orthographic representation (Ehri, 1992, 2005, 2017; Perfetti, 1992; Rahbari & Sénéchal, 2010; Share, 1999; Taha & Saiegh-Haddad, 2017; Tibi, 2016). As a further illustration, Table 1 shows examples of words derived from three different root structures.…”
Section: Arabic Morphologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Raw score effects may be misleading because English readers may show greater variation in both word and pseudoword reading than readers of more consistent orthographies, in particular when the consistent-orthography groups show ceiling effects in accuracy, as is often the case. In one of the few single-language studies investigating the development of the lexicality effect, Rahbari and Sénéchal (2010) contrasted first to fourth graders' reading efficiency of transparent (consistent) and opaque (inconsistent) words and pseudowords in the relatively consistent Farsi orthography. Taking the view that successful reading experience drives the lexicalization of printed words, which in turn facilitates word recognition, these authors predicted that children would demonstrate lexicality effects of increasing magnitude from second grade onward for words with the most transparent mappings, because transparency should accelerate the establishment of lexicalized representations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We expected acquired impairment in reading (dyslexia) to be co‐incident with impaired picture naming (anomia) and a dissociation whereby impairments would be observed with preserved comprehension. We also expected reading of opaque words to be more affected than transparent words by aphasia following studies of reading in Persian (Baluch, ; Baluch & Shahidi, ; Rahbari & Senechal, , ) including developmental dyslexia (Baluch & Danaye‐Tousi, ; Pouretemad, Khatibi, Zarei, & Stein, ). As in other Semitic scripts, we expected reading disorders to resemble the patterns of deep, phonological, and surface dyslexia already reported in Arabic and Hebrew (Beland & Mimouni, ; Friedman & Haddad‐Hanna, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%